Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 170, Issue 3, pp 302–311 | Cite as

Two eyes in action

  • Eli BrennerEmail author
  • Jeroen B. J. Smeets
Research Article

Abstract

Do relative binocular disparities guide our movements in depth? In order to find out we asked subjects to move a ‘cursor’ to a target within a simulated horizontal plane at eye height. They did so by moving a computer mouse. We determined how quickly subjects responded to the target jumping in depth. We found that it took subjects about 200 ms to respond to changes in binocular disparity. Subjects responded just as quickly if the cursor was temporarily only visible to one eye near the time that the target jumped in depth, and less vigorously, though just as quickly, if the cursor jumped rather than the target, so the fastest binocular responses cannot be based directly on the relative retinal disparity between the target and the cursor. Subjects reacted faster to changes in the target’s height in the visual field than to changes in binocular disparity, but did not react faster to changes in image size. These results suggest that binocular vision mainly improves people’s everyday movements by giving them a better sense of the distances of relevant objects, rather than by relative retinal disparities being used to directly guide the movement. We propose that relative disparities only guide parts of very slow movements that require extreme precision.

Keywords

Motor control Binocular vision Latency Disparity Prehension Stereopsis Distance Human 

References

  1. Bradshaw MF, Elliott KM (2003) The role of binocular information in the ‘on-line’ control of prehension. Spat Vis 16:295–309CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bradshaw MF, Hibbard PB (2003) Reaching for virtual objects: binocular disparity, retinal motion and the control of prehension. Arq Bras Oftalmol 66:53–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bradshaw MF, Elliott KM, Watt SJ, Hibbard PB, Davies IR, Simpson PJ (2004) Binocular cues and the control of prehension. Spat Vis 17:95–110CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Brenner E, van Damme W (1999) Perceived distance, shape and size. Vis Res 39:975–986CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (1997) Fast responses of the human hand to changes in target position. J Motor Behav 29:297–310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2000) Comparing extra-retinal information about distance and direction. Vis Res 40:1649–1651CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2001) We are better off without perfect perception. Behav Brain Sci 24:215CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2003) Fast corrections of movements with a computer mouse. Spat Vis 16:365–376CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Brenner E, van den Berg AV, van Damme WJ (1996) Perceived motion in depth. Vis Res 36:699–706CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Brenner E, Smeets JBJ, de Lussanet MHE (1998) Hitting moving targets: continuous control of the acceleration of the hand on the basis of the target’s velocity. Exp Brain Res 122:467–474CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Brenner E, Smeets JBJ, Landy MS (2001) How vertical disparities assist judgements of distance. Vis Res 41:3455–3465CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Cutting JE, Vishton PM (1995) Perceiving layout and knowing distances: the integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth. In: Epstein W, Rogers S (eds) Perception of space and motion. Academic, New York, pp 69–117Google Scholar
  13. Dijkerman HC, Milner AD (1998) The perception and prehension of objects oriented in the depth plane II Dissociated orientation functions in normal subjects. Exp Brain Res 118:408–414CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Dijkerman HC, Milner AD, Carey DP (1999) Motion parallax enables depth processing for action in a visual form agnosic when binocular vision is unavailable. Neuropsychologia 37:1505–1510CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Gardner PL, Mon-Williams M (2001) Vertical gaze angle: absolute height-in-scene information for the programming of prehension. Exp Brain Res 136:379–385CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Glennerster A, Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF (1996) Stereoscopic depth constancy depends on the subject’s task. Vis Res 36:3441–3456CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15:20–25CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Hibbard PB, Bradshaw MF (2003) Reaching for virtual objects: binocular disparity and the control of prehension. Exp Brain Res 148:196–201PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Jackson SR, Jones CA, Newport R, Pritchard C (1997) A kinematic analysis of goal-directed prehension movements executed under binocular, monocular, and memory-guided viewing conditions. Vis Cogn 4:113–142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jeannerod M (1986) The formation of finger grip during prehension A cortically mediated visuomotor pattern. Behav Brain Res 19:99–116CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Loftus A, Servos P, Goodale MA, Mendarozqueta N, Mon-Williams M (2004) When two eyes are better than one in prehension: monocular viewing and end-point variance. Exp Brain Res 158:317–327PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Marotta JJ, Goodale MA (1998) The role of learned pictorial cues in the programming and control of grasping. Exp Brain Res 121:465–470CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Marotta JJ, Kruyer A, Goodale MA (1998) The role of head movements in the control of manual prehension. Exp Brain Res 120:134–138CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Mazyn LI, Lenoir M, Montagne G, Savelsbergh GJ (2004) The contribution of stereo vision to one-handed catching. Exp Brain Res 157:383–390CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Milner AD, Paulignan Y, Dijkerman HC, Michel F, Jeannerod M (1999) A paradoxical improvement of misreaching in optic ataxia: new evidence for two separate neural systems for visual localization. Proc R Soc Biol Sci 266:2225–2229CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mon-Williams M, McIntosh RD, Milner AD (2001a) Vertical gaze angle as a distance cue for programming reaching: insights from visual form agnosia II (of III). Exp Brain Res 139:137–142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mon-Williams M, Tresilian JR, McIntosh RD, Milner AD (2001b) Monocular and binocular distance cues: insights from visual form agnosia I (of III). Exp Brain Res 139:127–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Morgan MJ (1989) Vision of solid objects. Nature 339:101–103CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Prablanc C, Martin O (1992) Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. J Neurophysiol 67:455–469PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Pélisson D, Prablanc C, Goodale MA, Jeannerod M (1986) Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb II Evidence of fast unconscious processes correcting the trajectory of the hand to the final position of a double-step stimulus. Exp Brain Res 62:303–311PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Rossetti Y (1998) Implicit short-lived motor representations of space in brain damaged and healthy subjects. Conscious Cogn 7:520–558CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Sarlegna F, Blouin J, Vercher JL, Bresciani JP, Bourdin C, Gauthier GM (2004) Online control of the direction of rapid reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 157:468–471CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Saunders JA, Knill DC (2003) Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 152:341–352CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Sedgwick HA (1986) Space perception. In: Boff KR, Kaufman L, Thomas JP (eds) Handbook of perception and human performance, vol 1. Sensory processes and perception. Wiley, New York, pp 21.1–21.57Google Scholar
  35. Servos P, Goodale MA (1994) Binocular vision and the on-line control of human prehension. Exp Brain Res 98:119–127CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Servos P, Goodale MA, Jakobson LS (1992) The role of binocular vision in prehension: a kinematic analysis. Vis Res 32:1513–1521CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Wann JP, Mon-Williams M, McIntosh RD, Smyth M, Milner AD (2001) The role of size and binocular information in guiding reaching: insights from virtual reality and visual form agnosia III (of III). Exp Brain Res 139:143–150CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Watt SJ, Bradshaw MF (2000) Binocular cues are important in controlling the grasp but not the reach in natural prehension movements. Neuropsychologia 38:1473–1481CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Watt SJ, Bradshaw MF (2002) Binocular information in the control of prehensile movements in multiple-object scenes. Spat Vis 15:141–155CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Watt SJ, Bradshaw MF (2003) The visual control of reaching and grasping: binocular disparity and motion parallax. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29:404–415CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of NeuroscienceErasmusMCRotterdamThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations