Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 157, Issue 1, pp 85–93 | Cite as

Sensory and response contributions to visual awareness in extinction

  • Raffaella Ricci
  • Anjan ChatterjeeEmail author
Research Article

Abstract.

Brain-damaged patients may extinguish contralesional stimuli when ipsilesional stimuli are presented simultaneously. Most theories of extinction postulate that stimuli compete for pathologically limited attentional resources with a bias to process ipsilesional over contralesional stimuli. Implicit in this view is the idea that responses follow the outcome of an earlier competition between inputs. In the current study of two patients, we used signal detection analyses to test the hypothesis that response criteria and response modalities also contribute to visual awareness. We found that identification was more sensitive than detection in uncovering deficits of contralesional awareness. Extinction was worse with bilateral stimuli when the ipsilesional stimulus was identical or similar to the target than when it was dissimilar. This diminished awareness was more likely to reflect a shift towards more conservative responses rather than diminished discrimination of contralesional stimuli. By contrast, one patient was better able to discriminate contralesional stimuli when using his contralesional limb to indicate awareness of targets than when using his ipsilesional limb. These data indicate that the nature of stimuli can modulate response criteria and the motor response can affect the sensory discriminability. Sensory discrimination and response output are not organized in a simple serial manner. Rather, input and output parameters interact in complicated ways to produce visual awareness. Visual awareness itself appears to be the outcome of two bottlenecks in processing, one having to do with sensory processing that may be covert and the other having to do with decision making, which by definition is overt. Finally, we advocate the use of signal detection analyses in studies of extinction, a method that has been surprisingly neglected in this line of research.

Keywords

Neglect Visual attention Consciousness Signal detection theory 

Notes

Acknowledgements.

This research was supported by NIH grant NS 037539. We thank Jessica Lease-Spellmeyer for help with data collection.

References

  1. Battersby WS, Bender MB, Pollack M, Kahn RN (1956) Unilateral spatial agnosia (inattention) in patients with cerebral lesions. Brain 79:68–93PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Baylis GC, Driver J, Rafal RD (1993) Visual extinction and stimulus repetition. J Cogn Neurosci 5:453–466Google Scholar
  3. Baylis GC, Gore CL, Rodriguez PD, Shisler RJ (2001) Visual extinction and awareness: the importance of binding dorsal and ventral pathways. Vis Cogn 8:359–379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bender MB (1952) Disorders in perception. CC Thomas, Springfield, ILGoogle Scholar
  5. Bisiach E (1999) Unilateral neglect and related disorders. In: Denes G, Pizzamiglio L (eds) Handbook of clinical and experimental neuropsychology. Psychology Press, Hove, East Sussex, pp 479–493Google Scholar
  6. Bisiach E, Vallar G (1988) Hemineglect in humans. In: Boller F, Grafman J (eds) Handbook of neuropsychology, vol 1. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 195–222Google Scholar
  7. Bisiach E, Vallar G, Geminiani G (1989) Influence of response modality on perceptual awareness of contralesional visual stimuli. Brain 1627–1636Google Scholar
  8. Bisiach E, Geminiani G, Berti A, Rusconi ML (1990) Perceptual and premotor factors of unilateral neglect. Neurology 40:1278–1281PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Bruning JL, Kintz BL (1977) Computational handbook of statistics. Scott Foresman and Co., Gelnnview, ILGoogle Scholar
  10. Chatterjee A (1998) Motor minds and mental models in neglect. Brain Cogn 37:339–349CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Chatterjee A (2002) Spatial anisometry and representational release in neglect. In: Karnath HO, Milner D, Vallar G (eds) The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  12. Chatterjee A, Mennemeier M, Heilman KM (1992) A stimulus-response relationship in unilateral neglect: the power function. Neuropsychologia 30:1101–1108CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Chatterjee A, Thompson KA, Ricci R (1999) Quantitative analysis of cancellation tasks in neglect. Cortex 35:153–162Google Scholar
  14. Colby CL, Goldberg GE (1999) Space and attention in parietal cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:319–349Google Scholar
  15. di Pellegrino G, De Renzi E (1995) An experimental investigation on the nature of extinction. Neuropsychologia 33:153–170CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Duhamel J, Brouchon M (1990) Sensorimotor aspects of unilateral neglect: a single case analysis. Cogn Neuropsychol 7:57–74Google Scholar
  17. Duncan J, Humphreys G, Ward R (1997) Competitive brain activity in visual attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol 7:255–261CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Farah MJ, Monheit MA, Wallace MA (1991) Unconscious perception of “extinguished” visual stimuli: reassessing the evidence. Neuropsychologia 29:949–958CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Grafton ST, Mazziotta JC, Woods RP, Phelps ME (1992) Human functional anatomy of visually guided finger movements. Brain 115:565–587PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Graziano MSA, Gross CG (1995) The representation of extrapersonal space: a possible role for bimodal, visual-tactile neurons. In: Gazzaniga MS (ed) The cognitive neurosciences. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 1021–1034Google Scholar
  21. Green DM, Swets JA (1966) Signal detection theory and psychophysics. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  22. Heilman KM, Valenstein E (1979) Mechanisms underlying hemispatial neglect. Ann Neurol 5:166–170PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Karnath H-O (1988) Deficits of attention in acute and recovered visual hemineglect. Neuropsychologia 34:9–22Google Scholar
  24. Kawashima R, Roland PE, O’Sullivan BT (1994) Fields in human motor areas involved in preparation for reaching, actual reaching, and visuomotor learning: a positron emission tomography study. J Neurosci 14:3462–3474PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Kinsbourne M (1993) Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In: Robertson IH, Marshall JC (eds) Unilateral neglect: clinical and experimental studies. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, pp 63–86Google Scholar
  26. Làdavas E, di Pellegrino G, Farnè A, Zeloni G (1998) Neuropsychological evidence of an integrated visuotactile representation of peripersonal space in humans. J Cogn Neurosci 10:581–589PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Mattingley J, Berberovic N, Rorden C, Driver J (2003) Attentional load at fixation modulates the spatial gradient for detection of visual stimuli in extinction. Cognitive Neuroscience Society, 10th Annual Meeting, March 30–April 1, New York, USA, p 125Google Scholar
  28. Milner AD, Harvey M, Roberts RC, Forster SV (1993) Line bisection errors in visual neglect: misguided action or size distortion? Neuropsychologia 31:39–49PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Na DL, Adair JC, Williamson DJG, Schwartz RL, Haws B, Heilman KM (1998) Dissociation of sensory-attentional from motor-intentional neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 64:331–338PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Olson E, Stark M, Chatterjee A (2003) Evidence for a unimodal somatosensory attention system. Exp Brain Res 151:15–23CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FJ, Rafal RD (1987) How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? Neuropsychologia 25:135–45PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Rafal R, Danziger S, Grossi G, Machado L, Ward R (2002) Visual detection is gated by attending for action: evidence from hemispatial neglect. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:16371–16375CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Rapcsak SZ, Verfaellie M, Fleet WS, Heilman KM (1989) Selective attention in hemispatial neglect. Arch Neurol 46:178–182PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Ricci R, Chatterjee A, Genero R, Zampieri D, Colombatti S (2003) Influence of sensory and response factors on contralesional visual awareness. Cognitive Neuroscience Society, 10th Annual Meeting, March 30–April 1, New York, USA, p 161Google Scholar
  35. Rizzolatti G, Camarda R (1987) Neural circuits for spatial attention and unilateral neglect. In: Jeannerod M (ed) Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of neglect. North Holland Press, Amsterdam, pp 289–313Google Scholar
  36. Robertson IH, North NT (1993) Active and passive activation of the left limbs: influence on visual and sensory neglect. Neuropsychologia 31:293–300CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Robertson IH, Tegnér R, Goodrich SJ, Wilson C (1994) Walking trajectory and hand movements in unilateral left neglect: a vestibular hypothesis. Neuropsychologia 32:1495–1502PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Smania N, Martini MC, Prior M, Marzi C (1996) Input and response determinants of visual extinction: a case study. Cortex 32:567–591PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Snodgrass JG, Corwin J (1988) Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. J Exp Psychol Gen 117:34–50PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Tegnér R, Levander M (1991) Through a looking glass. A new technique to demonstrate directional hypokinesia in unilateral neglect. Brain 114:1943–1951PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Vaishnavi S, Calhoun J, Chatterjee A (1999) Crossmodal and sensorimotor integration in tactile awareness. Neurology 53:1596–1598Google Scholar
  42. Vaishnavi S, Calhoun J, Southwood MH, Chatterjee A (2000) Sensory and response interference by ipsilesional stimulation in tactile extinction. Cortex 36:81–92PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Vaishnavi S, Calhoun J, Chatterjee A (2001) Binding personal and peripersonal space: evidence from tactile extinction. J Cogn Neurosci 13:181–189CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Vuilleumier P, Rafal RD (2000) A systematic study of visual extinction between- and within-field deficits of attention in hemispatial neglect. Brain 123:1263–1279CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Wilson B, Cockburn J, Halligan PW (1987) Behavioral Inattention Test. Thames Valley Test Co., Bury St. Edmunds, UKGoogle Scholar
  46. Worthington AD (1996) Cueing strategies in neglect dyslexia. Neuropsychol Rehabil 6:1–17Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Dipartimento di PsicologiaUniversità di TorinoTorinoItaly
  2. 2.Department of Neurology and Center for Cognitive NeuroscienceUniversity of PennsylvaniaPhiladelphiaUSA
  3. 3.PhiladelphiaUSA

Personalised recommendations