Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 155, Issue 3, pp 334–340 | Cite as

Internal models underlying grasp can be additively combined

Research Article

Abstract

Our ability to additively combine two learned internal models was investigated by studying the forces people generate when lifting objects with a precision grip. Subjects were required to alternately lift two objects of identical physical appearance but differing weight. Grip force scaling prior to lift-off was used to estimate the output of the internal model associated with each object. Appropriate internal models were formed when alternately lifting two objects of different weight. The objects were then combined by stacking them one upon the other, and the combined object was lifted. Results show that subjects can additively combine internal models of object dynamics but the sum is biased by a default estimate of the object’s weight.

Keywords

Precision grip Motor control Motor programming Internal models Prediction 

References

  1. Brashers-Krug T, Shadmehr R, Bizzo E (1996) Consolidation in human motor memory. Nature 382:252–255PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Burstedt MK, Edin BB, Johansson RS (1997) Coordination of fingertip forces during human manipulation can emerge from independent neural networks controlling each engaged digit. Exp Brain Res 117:67–79CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Edin BB, Westling G, Johansson RS (1992) Independent control of human finger-tip forces at individual digits during precision lifting. J Physiol 450:547–564PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Flanagan JR, Beltzner MA (2000) Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size-weight illusion. Nat Neurosci 3:737–741CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Flanagan JR, Wing AM (1995) The stability of precision grip forces during cyclic arm movements with a hand-held load. Exp Brain Res 105:455–464PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Flanagan JR, Nakano E, Imamizu H, Osu R, Yoshioka T, Kawato M (1999) Composition and decomposition of internal models in motor learning under altered kinematic and dynamic environments. J Neurosci 19: RC34PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Gandolfo F, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Bizzi E (1996) Motor learning by field approximation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93:3843–3846Google Scholar
  8. Ghahramani Z, Wolpert DM (1997) Modular decomposition in visuomotor learning. Nature 386:392–395PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Gordon AM, Forssberg H, Johansson RS, Westling G (1991a) The integration of haptically acquired size information in the programming of precision grip. Exp Brain Res 83:483–488PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Gordon AM, Forssberg H, Johansson RS, Westling G (1991b) Visual size cues in the programming of manipulative forces during precision grip. Exp Brain Res 83:477–482PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Gordon AM, Westling G, Cole KJ, Johansson RS (1993) Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. J Neurophysiol 69:1789–1796PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Haruno M, Wolpert DM, Kawato M (2001) Mosaic model for sensorimotor learning and control. Neural Comput 13:2201–2220CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Jenmalm P, Goodwin AW, Johansson RS (1998) Control of grasp stability when humans lift objects with different surface curvatures. J Neurophysiol 79:1643–1652PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Johansson RS, Westling G (1984) Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects. Exp Brain Res 56:550–564PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Johansson RS, Westling G (1988) Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately and erroneously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip. Exp Brain Res 71:59–71PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Karniel A, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2002) Does the motor control system use multiple models and context switching to cope with a variable environment? Exp Brain Res 143:520–524PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Krakauer JW, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (1999) Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching. Nature Neuroscience 2:1026–1031CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Murray DJ, Ellis RR, Bandomir CA, Ross HE (1999) Charpentier (1891) on the size-weight illusion. Percept Psychophys 61:1681–1685PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Neilson PD, Neilson MD, O’Dwyer NJ (1985) Acquisition of motor skills in tracking tasks: Learning internal models. In: Russell DG, Abernethy B (eds) Motor memory and control. Human Performance Associates, Dunedin, pp 25–36Google Scholar
  20. Quaney BM, Rotella DL, Peterson C, Cole KJ (2003) Sensorimotor memory for fingertip forces: evidence for a task-independent motor memory. J Neurosci 23:1981–1986PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Witney AG, Vetter P, Wolpert DM (2001) The influence of previous experience on predictive motor control. Neuroreport 12:649–653PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Wolpert DM, Kawato M (1998) Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks 11:1317–1329CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Sobell Dept of Motor Neuroscience, Institute of NeurologyUniversity College LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations