Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 153, Issue 1, pp 59–75

Bimanual aiming and overt attention: one law for two hands

  • S. Riek
  • J. R. Tresilian
  • M. Mon-Williams
  • V. L. Coppard
  • R. G. Carson
Research Article

Abstract.

Reaching to interact with an object requires a compromise between the speed of the limb movement and the required end-point accuracy. The time it takes one hand to move to a target in a simple aiming task can be predicted reliably from Fitts' law, which states that movement time is a function of a combined measure of amplitude and accuracy constraints (the index of difficulty, ID). It has been assumed previously that Fitts' law is violated in bimanual aiming movements to targets of unequal ID. We present data from two experiments to show that this assumption is incorrect: if the attention demands of a bimanual aiming task are constant then the movements are well described by a Fitts' law relationship. Movement time therefore depends not only on ID but on other task conditions, which is a basic feature of Fitts' law. In a third experiment we show that eye movements are an important determinant of the attention demands in a bimanual aiming task. The results from the third experiment extend the findings of the first two experiments and show that bimanual aiming often relies on the strategic co-ordination of separate actions into a seamless behaviour. A number of the task specific strategies employed by the adult human nervous system were elucidated in the third experiment. The general strategic pattern observed in the hand trajectories was reflected by the pattern of eye movements recorded during the experiment. The results from all three experiments demonstrate that eye movements must be considered as an important constraint in bimanual aiming tasks.

Keywords

Movement Planning Preparation Prehension 

References

  1. Binsted G, Chua R, Helsen W, Elliottt D (2001) Eye-hand coordination in goal-directed aiming. Hum Mov Sci 20:563–585CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bizzi E, Kalil RE, Tagliasco V (1971) Eye-hand coordination in monkeys: evidence for centrally patterned organisation. Science 173:452–454Google Scholar
  3. Carlton LG (1981) Visual information: the control of aiming movements. Q J Exp Psychol 33A:87–93Google Scholar
  4. Carson RG (1989) Manual asymmetries: In defense of a multifactorial account. J Mot Behav 21:157–162Google Scholar
  5. Cohen J (1969) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  6. Corbetta D, Thelen E (1996) The developmental origins of bimanual coordination: A dynamic perspective. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22:502–522CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Corcos DM (1984) Two-handed movement control. Res Q Ex Sport 55:117–122Google Scholar
  8. Elliott D (1992) Intermittent versus continuous control of manual aiming movements. In: Proteau L, Elliott D (eds) Vision and motor control. North-Holland, Amsterdam, pp 3–31Google Scholar
  9. Elliott D, Lyons J, Chua R, Goodman D, Carson RG (1995) The influence of target perturbation on manual asymmetries in right-handers. Cortex 31:133–154Google Scholar
  10. Fitts PM, Peterson JR (1964) Information capacity of discrete motor responses. J Exp Psychol 67:103–112Google Scholar
  11. Fowler B, Duck T, Mosher M, Mathieson B (1991) The coordination of bimanual aiming movements: evidence for progressive desynchronization. Q J Exp Psychol A43:205–221Google Scholar
  12. Helsen WF, Starkes JL, Elliott D, Buekers MJ (1998) Manual asymmetries and saccadic eye movements in right-handers during single and reciprocal aiming movements. Cortex 34:513–529PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Helsen WF, Starkes JL, Elliott D, Ricker KL (2000) Coupling of eye, finger, elbow and shoulder movements during manual aiming. J Mot Behav 32:241–248PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Hering E (1868/1977) The theory of binocular vision. Plenum Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  15. Honda H (1982) Rightward superiority of eye movements in a bimanual aiming task. Q J Exp Psychol 34A:499–513Google Scholar
  16. Kelso JAS, Schöner G (1988) Self-organisation of coordinative movement patterns. Hum Mov Sci 7:27–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kelso JAS, Southard DL, Goodman D (1979a) On the nature of human interlimb coordination. Science 203:1029–1031Google Scholar
  18. Kelso JAS, Southard DL, Goodman D (1979b) On the coordination of two-handed movements. J Exp Psychol 5:229–238PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Marteniuk RG, Mackenzie CL, Baba DM (1984) Bimanual movement control: information processing and interaction effects. Q J Exp Psychol A36:335–365Google Scholar
  20. Peters M (1981) Attentional asymmetries during concurrent bimanual performance. Q J Exp Psychol A33:95–103Google Scholar
  21. Robinson GH, Kavinsky RC (1976) On Fitts' law with two handed movements. IEEE Trans Syst Man Cybern 6:504–505Google Scholar
  22. Roy EA, Elliott D (1989) Manual asymmetries in aimed movements. Q J Exp Psychol 41A:501–516Google Scholar
  23. Schmidt RA (1988) Motor control and learning: a behavioural emphasis, 2nd edn. Human Kinetics, ILGoogle Scholar
  24. Steenbergen B, Hulstijn W, de Vries A, Berger M (1996) Bimanual movement coordination in spastic hemiparesis. Exp Brain Res 110:91–98PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Sugden D, Utley A (1995) Interlimb coupling in children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 37:293–309PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Teasdale N, Bard C, Fleury M, Young DE, Proteau L (1993) Determining movement onsets from temporal series. J Mot Behav 25:97–106Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  • S. Riek
    • 1
  • J. R. Tresilian
    • 1
  • M. Mon-Williams
    • 2
  • V. L. Coppard
    • 1
  • R. G. Carson
    • 1
  1. 1.School of Human Movement StudiesUniversity of QueenslandSt. LuciaAustralia
  2. 2.School of Psychology Mathematics and ComputingUniversity of AberdeenAberdeenScotland

Personalised recommendations