Psychopharmacology

, Volume 235, Issue 4, pp 947–958 | Cite as

A caffeine-maltodextrin mouth rinse counters mental fatigue

  • Jeroen Van Cutsem
  • Kevin De Pauw
  • Samuele Marcora
  • Romain Meeusen
  • Bart Roelands
Original Investigation

Abstract

Introduction

Mental fatigue is a psychobiological state caused by prolonged periods of demanding cognitive activity that has negative implications on many aspects in daily life. Caffeine and carbohydrate ingestion have been shown to be able to reduce these negative effects of mental fatigue. Intake of these substances might however be less desirable in some situations (e.g., restricted caloric intake, Ramadan). Rinsing caffeine or glucose within the mouth has already been shown to improve exercise performance. Therefore, we sought to evaluate the effect of frequent caffeine-maltodextrin (CAF-MALT) mouth rinsing on mental fatigue induced by a prolonged cognitive task.

Methods

Ten males (age 23 ± 2 years, physical activity 7.3 ± 4.3 h/week, low CAF users) performed two trials. Participants first completed a Flanker task (3 min), then performed a 90-min mentally fatiguing task (Stroop task), followed by another Flanker task. Before the start and after each 12.5% of the Stroop task (eight blocks), subjects received a CAF-MALT mouth rinse (MR: 0.3 g/25 ml CAF: 1.6g/25 ml MALT) or placebo (PLAC: 25 ml artificial saliva).

Results

Self-reported mental fatigue was lower in MR (p = 0.017) compared to PLAC. Normalized accuracy (accuracy first block = 100%) was higher in the last block of the Stroop in MR (p = 0.032) compared to PLAC. P2 amplitude in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) decreased over time only in PLAC (p = 0.017).

Conclusion

Frequent mouth rinsing during a prolonged and demanding cognitive task reduces mental fatigue compared to mouth rinsing with artificial saliva.

Keywords

Cognitive fatigue Mouth rinse Electroencephalography Cognitive performance 

Notes

Acknowledgments

BR is a postdoctoral fellow of the Fund for Scientific Research Flanders (FWO).

Compliance with ethical standards

Each subject gave written informed consent prior to the study. Experimental protocol and procedures were approved by the Research Council of the Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Belgium.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Aeschbach D, Matthews JR, Postolache TT, Jackson MA, Giesen HA, Wehr TA (1997) Dynamics of the human EEG during prolonged wakefulness: evidence for frequency-specific circadian and homeostatic influences. Neurosci Lett 239(2-3):121–124.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(97)00904-X CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Beaven CM, Maulder P, Pooley A, Kilduff L, Cook C (2013) Effects of caffeine and carbohydrate mouth rinses on repeated sprint performance. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 38(6):633–637.  https://doi.org/10.1139/apnm-2012-0333 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bland JM, Altman DG (1995) Calculating correlation coefficients with repeated observations: part 1--correlation within subjects. BMJ 310(6977):446.  https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.310.6977.446 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Boksem MA, Meijman TF, Lorist MM (2005) Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study Brain. Res Cogn Brain Res 25(1):107–116.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boksem MA, Tops M (2008) Mental fatigue: costs and benefits. Brain Res Rev 59(1):125–139.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.07.001 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bower JE (2014) Cancer-related fatigue—mechanisms, risk factors, and treatments. Nat Rev Clin Oncol 11(10):597–609.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrclinonc.2014.127 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Brownsberger J, Edwards A, Crowther R, Cottrell D (2013) Impact of mental fatigue on self-paced exercise. Int J Sports Med 34(12):1029–1036.  https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0033-1343402 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Cajochen C, Brunner DP, Kräuchi K, Graw P, Wirz-Justice A (1995) Power density in theta/alpha frequencies of the waking EEG progressively increases during sustained wakefulness. Sleep 18(10):890–894.  https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/18.10.890 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Carter JM, Jeukendrup AE, Jones DA (2004) The effect of carbohydrate mouth rinse on 1-h cycle time trial performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc 36(12):2107–2111CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Chambers ES, Bridge MW, Jones DA (2009) Carbohydrate sensing in the human mouth: effects on exercise performance and brain activity. J Physiol 587(8):1779–1794.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2008.164285 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Chaudhuri A, Behan PO (2004) Fatigue in neurological disorders. Lancet 363(9413):978–988.  https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(04)15794-2 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Childs E (2014) Influence of energy drink ingredients on mood and cognitive performance. Nutr Rev 72 Suppl 1:48–59.  https://doi.org/10.1111/nure.12148 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Cook DB, O'Connor PJ, Lange G, Steffener J (2007) Functional neuroimaging correlates of mental fatigue induced by cognition among chronic fatigue syndrome patients and controls. NeuroImage 36(1):108–122.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.033 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. de Araujo IE, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, McGlone F (2003) Human cortical responses to water in the mouth, and the effects of thirst. J Neurophysiol 90(3):1865–1876.  https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00297.2003 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. de Ataide e Silva T, Di Cavalcanti Alves de Souza ME, de Amorim JF, Stathis CG, Leandro CG, Lima-Silva AE (2013) Can carbohydrate mouth rinse improve performance during exercise? A systematic review Nutrients 6(1):1–10.  https://doi.org/10.3390/nu6010001
  16. De Pauw K, Roelands B, Knaepen K, Polfliet M, Stiens J, Meeusen R (2015) Effects of caffeine and maltodextrin mouth rinsing on P300, brain imaging, and cognitive performance. J Appl Physiol (1985) 118(6):776–782.  https://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.01050.2014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Desmond PA, Hancock PA (2001) Active and passive fatigue states. In: PA H, PA D (eds) Stress, workload and fatigue. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, pp 455–465Google Scholar
  18. Doering TM, Fell JW, Leveritt MD, Desbrow B, Shing CM (2014) The effect of a caffeinated mouth-rinse on endurance cycling time-trial performance. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab 24(1):90–97.  https://doi.org/10.1123/ijsnem.2013-0103 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Folstein JR, Van Petten C (2008) Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. Psychophysiology 45:152–170.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Frank GK, Oberndorfer TA, Simmons AN, Paulus MP, Fudge JL, Yang TT, Kaye WH (2008) Sucrose activates human taste pathways differently from artificial sweetener. NeuroImage 39(4):1559–1569.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.061 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Gajewski PD, Falkenstein M (2015) Long-term habitual physical activity is associated with lower distractibility in a Stroop interference task in aging: behavioral and ERP evidence. Brain Cogn 98:87–101.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2015.06.004 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Gajewski PD, Stoerig P, Falkenstein M (2008) ERP—correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm. Brain Res 1189:127–134.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.076 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Gergelyfi M, Jacob B, Olivier E, Zenon A (2015) Dissociation between mental fatigue and motivational state during prolonged mental activity. Front Behav Neurosci 9:176.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00176 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Glade MJ (2010) Caffeine—not just a stimulant. Nutrition 26(10):932–938.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nut.2010.08.004 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Guay F, Vallerand RJ, Blanchard C (2000) On the assessment of situational intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: the Situational Motivation Scale (SIMS). Motiv Emotion 24:175–213.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005614228250 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Haase L, Cerf-Ducastel B, Murphy C (2009) Cortical activation in response to pure taste stimuli during the physiological states of hunger and satiety. NeuroImage 44(3):1008–1021.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.044 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis NL (2013) The sweet taste of success: the presence of glucose in the oral cavity moderates the depletion of self-control resources. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 39(1):28–42.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212459912 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hart SG, Staveland LE (1988) Development of NASA-TLX (task load index): results of empirical and theoretical research Human. Ment Workload 1:139–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Jasper HH (1958) Report of the committee on methods of clinical examination in electroencephalography. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 10:370–371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Job RFS, Dalziel J (2001) Defining fatigue as a condition of the organism and distinguishing it from habituation, adaptation and boredom. In: P.A.H.P.A. Desmond E (ed) stress, workload and fatigue. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, pp 466–475Google Scholar
  31. Kumar N, Wheaton LA, Snow TK, Millard-Stafford M (2016) Carbohydrate ingestion but not mouth rinse maintains sustained attention when fasted. Physiol Behav 153:33–39.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.10.023 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Lal SK, Craig A (2002) Driver fatigue: electroencephalography and psychological assessment. Psychophysiology 39(3):313–321.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0048577201393095 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Lieberman HR (2001) The effects of ginseng, ephedrine, and caffeine on cognitive performance, mood and energy. Nutr Rev 59(4):91–102CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Lorist MM (2008) Impact of top-down control during mental fatigue. Brain Res 1232:113–123.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.053 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Lorist MM, Tops M (2003) Caffeine, fatigue, and cognition. Brain Cogn 53:82–94CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. MacLeod CM, MacDonald PA (2000) Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends Cogn Sci 4(10):383–391.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01530-8 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Malik VS, Popkin BM, Bray GA, Després JP, FB H (2010) Sugar-sweetened beverages, obesity, type 2 diabetes mellitus, and cardiovascular disease risk. Circulation 121(11):1356–1364.  https://doi.org/10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.109.876185 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. Marcora SM, Staiano W, Manning V (2009) Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. J Appl Physiol (1985) 106(3):857–864.  https://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.91324.2008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. McCormick F, Kadzielski J, Landrigan CP, Evans B, Herndon JH, Rubash HE (2012) Surgeon fatigue: a prospective analysis of the incidence, risk, and intervals of predicted fatigue-related impairment in residents. Arch Surg 147(5):430–435.  https://doi.org/10.1001/archsurg.2012.84 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. McLellan TM, Caldwell JA, Lieberman HR (2016) A review of caffeine’s effects on cognitive, physical and occupational performance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 71:294–312.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.001 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Mostofsky SH, Simmonds DJ (2008) Response inhibition and response selection: two sides of the same coin. J Cogn Neurosci 20(5):751–761.  https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20500 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. O'Doherty J et al (2000) Sensory-specific satiety-related olfactory activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroreport 11(4):893–897.  https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200003200-00046 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Pageaux B, Marcora SM, Rozand V, Lepers R (2015) Mental fatigue induced by prolonged self-regulation does not exacerbate central fatigue during subsequent whole-body endurance exercise. Front Hum Neurosci 9:67.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00067 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. Phipps-Nelson J, Redman JR, Rajaratnam SM (2011) Temporal profile of prolonged, night-time driving performance: breaks from driving temporarily reduce time-on-task fatigue but not sleepiness. J Sleep Res 20(3):404–415.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00900.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Polich J (2007) Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol 118:2128–2148.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Rollo I, Cole M, Miller R, Williams C (2010) Influence of mouth rinsing a carbohydrate solution on 1-h running performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc 42(4):798–804.  https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181bac6e4 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Rollo I, Williams C (2011) Effect of mouth-rinsing carbohydrate solutions on endurance performance. Sports Med 41(6):449–461.  https://doi.org/10.2165/11588730-000000000-00000 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Shargel L, Yu A (2016) Applied biopharmaceutics & pharmacokinetics. 7th edn. Mc Graw Hill EducationGoogle Scholar
  49. Smith MR, Coutts AJ, Merlini M, Deprez D, Lenoir M, Marcora SM (2016) Mental fatigue impairs soccer-specific physical and technical performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc 48(2):267–276.  https://doi.org/10.1249/mss.0000000000000762 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Tops M, Boksem MA (2010) Absorbed in the task: personality measures predict engagement during task performance as tracked by error negativity and asymmetrical frontal activity. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 10(4):441–453.  https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.10.4.441 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Trejo LJ, Kubitz K, Rosipal R, Kochavi RL, Montgomery LD (2015) EEG-based estimation and classification of mental fatigue. Psychology 6(05):572–589.  https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2015.65055 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Turner CE, Byblow WD, Stinear CM, Gant N (2014) Carbohydrate in the mouth enhances activation of brain circuitry involved in motor performance and sensory perception. Appetite 80:212–219.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.05.020 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Van Cutsem J, Marcora S, De Pauw K, Bailey S, Meeusen R, Roelands B (2017) The effects of mental fatigue on physical performance: a systematic review. Sports Med 47(8):1569–1588.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-016-0672-0 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. van der Linden D, Massar SA, Schellekens AF, Ellenbroek BA, Verkes RJ (2006) Disrupted sensorimotor gating due to mental fatigue: preliminary evidence. Int J Psychophysiol 62(1):168–174.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.04.001 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Wald F, Mellenbergh G (1990) De verkorte versie van de Nederlandse vertaling van de Profile of Mood States (POMS) [The shortened version of the Dutch translation of the Profile of Mood States (POMS)] Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie 45:86-90Google Scholar
  56. Wascher E, Rasch B, Sänger J, Hoffmann S, Schneider D, Rinkenauer G, Heuer H, Gutberlet I (2014) Frontal theta activity reflects distinct aspects of mental fatigue. Biol Psychol 96:57–65.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.010 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Weeks SR, McAuliffe CL, Durussel D, Pasquina PF (2010) Physiological and psychological fatigue in extreme conditions: the military example. PM R 2(5):438–441.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmrj.2010.03.023 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Wehrwein EA, Carter JR (2016) The mind matters: psychology as an overlooked variable within physiology studies. Physiology (Bethesda) 31(2):74–75.  https://doi.org/10.1152/physiol.00059.2015 Google Scholar
  59. Wolk BJ, Ganetsky M, Babu KM (2012) Toxicity of energy drinks. Curr Opin Pediatr 24(2):243–251.  https://doi.org/10.1097/MOP.0b013e3283506827 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Zhao C, Zhao M, Liu J, Zheng C (2012) Electroencephalogram and electrocardiograph assessment of mental fatigue in a driving simulator. Accid Anal Prev 45:83–90.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2011.11.019 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Human Physiology Research GroupVrije Universiteit BrusselBrusselsBelgium
  2. 2.Endurance Research Group, School of Sport and Exercise SciencesUniversity of Kent at MedwayKentUK

Personalised recommendations