Advertisement

Psychopharmacology

, Volume 231, Issue 24, pp 4735–4744 | Cite as

The beneficial effect of oxytocin on avoidance-related facial emotion recognition depends on early life stress experience

  • Melanie Feeser
  • Yan Fan
  • Anne Weigand
  • Adam Hahn
  • Matti Gärtner
  • Sabine Aust
  • Heinz Böker
  • Malek Bajbouj
  • Simone Grimm
Original Investigation

Abstract

Rationale

Previous studies have shown that oxytocin (OXT) enhances social cognitive processes. It has also been demonstrated that OXT does not uniformly facilitate social cognition. The effects of OXT administration strongly depend on the exposure to stressful experiences in early life. Emotional facial recognition is crucial for social cognition. However, no study has yet examined how the effects of OXT on the ability to identify emotional faces are altered by early life stress (ELS) experiences. Given the role of OXT in modulating social motivational processes, we specifically aimed to investigate its effects on the recognition of approach- and avoidance-related facial emotions.

Methods

In a double-blind, between-subjects, placebo-controlled design, 82 male participants performed an emotion recognition task with faces taken from the “Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces” set. We clustered the six basic emotions along the dimensions approach (happy, surprise, anger) and avoidance (fear, sadness, disgust). ELS was assessed with the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ).

Results

Our results showed that OXT improved the ability to recognize avoidance-related emotional faces as compared to approach-related emotional faces. Whereas the performance for avoidance-related emotions in participants with higher ELS scores was comparable in both OXT and placebo condition, OXT enhanced emotion recognition in participants with lower ELS scores. Independent of OXT administration, we observed increased emotion recognition for avoidance-related faces in participants with high ELS scores.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that the investigation of OXT on social recognition requires a broad approach that takes ELS experiences as well as motivational processes into account.

Keywords

Oxytocin Neuropeptide Facial emotion recognition Early life stress 

Notes

Conflicts of interest

All authors report no potential conflict of interest or financial interest.

References

  1. Adams RB Jr, Kleck RE (2005) Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion 5(1):3–11PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Adophs R (2002) Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr Opin Neurobiol 12:169–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Andari E, Duhamel JR, Zalla T, Herbrecht E, Leboyer M, Sirigu A (2010) Promoting social behavior with oxytocin in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107(9):4389–4394PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH (2013) Sniffing around oxytocin: review and meta-analyses of trials in healthy and clinical groups with implications for pharmacotherapy. Transl Psychiatry 3:e258PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bartz JA, Hollander E (2006) The neuroscience of affiliation: forging links between basic and clinical research on neuropeptides and social behavior. Horm Behav 50:518–528PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N, Ochsner KN (2011) Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn Sci 15:301–309PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Baumgartner T, Heinrichs M, Vonlanthen A, Fischbacher U, Fehr E (2008) Oxytocin shapes the neural circuitry of trust and trust adaptation in humans. Neuron 58:639–650PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bernstein DP, Fink L (1998) Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: a retrospective self-report manual. The Psychological Corporation, San AntonioGoogle Scholar
  9. Bielsky IF, Young LJ (2004) Oxytocin, vasopressin, and social recognition in mammals. Peptides 25(9):1565–1574PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bromberg-Martin ES, Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O (2010) Dopamine in motivational control: rewarding, aversive, and alerting. Neuron 68:815–834PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Campbell A (2010) Oxytocin and human social behavior. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 14(3):281–295CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Carter CS, Grippo AJ, Pournajafi-Nazarloo H, Ruscio MG, Porges SW (2008) Oxytocin, vasopressin and sociality. Prog Brain Res 170:331–336PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Costa PTM (1992) Revised NEO personality inventory and NEO five-factor inventory (Professional Manual). Psychological Assessment Resources, OdessaGoogle Scholar
  14. Dannlowski U, Stuhrmann A, Beutelmann V, Zwanzger P, Lenzen T, Grotegerd D, Domschke K, Hohoff C, Ohrmann P, Bauer J, Lindner C, Postert C, Konrad C, Arolt V, Heindel W, Suslow T, Kugel H (2012) Limbic scars: long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biol Psychiatry 71(4):286–293PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. De Dreu CKW, Greer LL, Handgraaf MJJ, Shalvi S, Van Kleef GA, Baas M, Ten Velden FS, Van Dijk E, Feith SW (2010) The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates parochial altruism in intergroup conflict among humans. Science 328:1408–1411PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Di Simplicio M, Massey-Chase R, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ (2009) Oxytocin enhances processing of positive versus negative emotional information in healthy male volunteers. J Psychopharmacol 23:241–248PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Gläscher J, Büchel C, Braus DF, Herpertz SC (2007a) Oxytocin attenuates amygdala responses to emotional faces regardless of valence. Biol Psychiatry 62:1187–1190PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Michel A, Berger C, Herpertz SC (2007b) Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biol Psychiatry 61(6):731–733PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Domes G, Lischke A, Berger C, Grossmann A, Hauenstein K, Heinrichs M, Herpertz SC (2010) Effects of intranasal oxytocin on emotional face processing in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:83–93PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Donaldson ZR, Young LJ (2008) Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science 322(5903):900–904PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Elliot AJ, Covington MV (2001) Approach and avoidance motivation. Educ Psychol Rev 13(2):73–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Feldman R, Weller A, Zagoory-Sharon O, Levine A (2007) Evidence for a neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation – plasma oxytocin levels across pregnancy and the postpartum period predict mother–infant bonding. Psychol Sci 18:965–970PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Fischer-Shofty M, Shamay-Tsoory SG, Harari H, Levkovitz Y (2010) The effect of intranasal administration of oxytocin on fear recognition. Neuropsychologia 48:179–184PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Francis DD, Champagne FC, Meaney MJ (2000) Variations in maternal behaviour are associated with differences in oxytocin receptor levels in the rat. J Neuroendocrinol 12(12):1145–1148PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Fries AB, Ziegler TE, Kurian JR, Jacoris S, Pollak SD (2005) Early experience in humans is associated with changes in neuropeptides critical for regulating social behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:17237–17240PubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gamer M, Büchel C (2012) Oxytocin specifically enhances valence-dependent parasympathetic responses. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37(1):87–93PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Goeleven E, Raedt RD, Leyman L, Verschuere B (2008) The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces: a validation study. Cogn Emot 22:1094–1118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Grant MM, Cannistraci C, Hollon SD, Gore J, Shelton R (2011) Childhood trauma history differentiates amygdala response to sad faces within MDD. J Psychiatr Res 45(7):886–895PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Groppe SE, Gossen A, Rademacher L, Hahn A, Westphal L, Gründer G, Spreckelmeyer KN (2013) Oxytocin influences processing of socially relevant cues in the ventral tegmental area of the human Brain. Biol Psychiatry 74(3):172–179PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Guastella AJ, Mitchell PB, Dadds MR (2008a) Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biol Psychiatry 63(1):3–5PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Guastella AJ, Mitchell PB, Mathews F (2008b) Oxytocin enhances the encoding of positive social memories in humans. Biol Psychiatry 64(3):256–258PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Harmon-Jones E, Gable PA, Peterson CK (2010) The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: a review and update. Biol Psychol 84(3):451–462PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Heim C, Young LJ, Newport DJ, Mletzko T, Miller AH, Nemeroff CB (2009) Lower CSF oxytocin concentrations in women with a history of childhood abuse. Mol Psychiatry 14:954–958PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Heinrichs M, Baumgartner T, Kirschbaum C, Ehlert U (2003) Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biol Psychiatry 54:1389–1398PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Heinrichs M, von Dawans B, Domes G (2009) Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol 30(4):548–557PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hurlemann R, Patin A, Onur OA, Cohen MX, Baumgartner T, Metzler S, Dziobek I, Gallinat J, Wagner M, Maier W, Kendrick KM (2010) Oxytocin enhances amygdala-dependent, socially reinforced learning and emotional empathy in humans. J Neurosci 30(14):4999–5007PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Irwin JR, McClelland GH (2003) Negative consequences of dichotomizing continuous predictor variables. J Mark Res 40(3):366–371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Judd CM, McClelland GH, Ryan CR (2009) Data analysis: a model comparison approach, 2nd ed. RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
  39. Kemp AH, Guastella AJ (2010) Oxytocin: prosocial behavior, social salience, or approach-related behavior? Biol Psychiatry 67:e33–e34, author reply e35PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kemp AH, Guastella AJ (2011) The role of Oxytocin in human affect: a novel hypothesis. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 20:222–231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kirsch P, Esslinger C, Chen Q, Mier D, Lis S, Siddhanti S, Gruppe H, Mattay VS, Gallhofer B, Meyer-Lindenberg A (2005) Oxytocin modulates neural circuitry for social cognition and fear in humans. J Neurosci 25(49):11489–11493PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E (2005) Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 435:673–676PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Lehrl S, Triebig G, Fischer B (1995) Multiple choice vocabulary test MWT as a valid and short test to estimate premorbid intelligence. Acta Neurol Scand 91:335–345PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Leist T, Dadds MR (2009) Adolescents’ ability to read different emotional faces relates to their history of maltreatment and type of psychopathology. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry 14(2):237–250PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Lischke A, Berger C, Prehn K, Heinrichs M, Herpertz SC, Domes G (2012a) Intranasal oxytocin enhances emotion recognition from dynamic facial expressions and leaves eye-gaze unaffected. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:475–481PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Lischke A, Gamer M, Berger C, Grossmann A, Hauenstein K, Heinrichs M, Herpertz SC, Domes G (2012b) Oxytocin increases amygdala reactivity to threatening scenes in females. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:1431–1438PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Loup F, Tribollet E, Dubois-Dauphin M, Dreifuss JJ (1991) Localization of high-affinity binding sites for oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain. An autoradiographic study. Brain Res 555:220–232PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Luminet O, Grynberg D, Ruzette N, Mikolajczak M (2011) Personality-dependent effects of oxytocin: greater social benefits for high alexithymia scorers. Biol Psychol 87:401–406PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Öhman A (1998) The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces — KDEF, CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet, ISBN 91-630-7164-9Google Scholar
  50. Maheu FS, Dozier M, Guyer AE, Mandell D, Peloso E, Poeth K, Jenness J, Lau JY, Ackerman JP, Pine DS, Ernst M (2010) A preliminary study of medial temporal lobe function in youths with a history of caregiver deprivation and emotional neglect. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 10(1):34–49PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Marsh AA, Yu HH, Pine DS, Blair RJ (2010) Oxytocin improves specific recognition of positive facial expressions. Psychopharmacology 209:225–232PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Masten CL, Guyer AE, Hodgdon HB, McClure EB, Charney DS, Ernst M, Kaufman J, Pine DS, Monk CS (2008) Recognition of facial emotions among maltreated children with high rates of post-traumatic stress disorder. Child Abuse Negl 32:139–153PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Meinlschmidt G, Heim C (2007) Sensitivity to intranasal Oxytocin in adult men with early parental separation. Biol Psychiatry 61(9):1109–1111PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Melis MR, Melis T, Cocco C, Succu S, Sanna F, Pillolla G, Boi A, Ferri GL, Argiolas A (2007) Oxytocin injected into the ventral tegmental area induces penile erection and increases extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens and paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus of male rats. Eur J Neurosci 26:1026–1035PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Domes G, Kirsch P, Heinrichs M (2011) Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nat Rev Neurosci 12(9):524–538PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Mikolajczak M, Pinon N, Lane A, de Timary P, Luminet O (2010) Oxytocin not only increases trust when money is at stake, but also when confidential information is in the balance. Biol Psychol 85:182–184PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Olff M, Frijling JL, Kubzansky LD, Bradley B, Ellenbogen MA, Cardoso C, Bartz JA, Yee JR, van Zuiden M (2013) The role of oxytocin in social bonding, stress regulation and mental health: an update on the moderating effects of context and interindividual differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38:1883–1894PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Petrovic P, Kalisch R, Singer T, Dolan RJ (2008) Oxytocin attenuates affective evaluations of conditioned faces and amygdala activity. J Neurosci 28:6607–6615PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Pollak SD, Kistler DJ (2002) Early experience is associated with the development of categorical representations for facial expressions of emotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99(13):9072–9076PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Pollak SD, Messner M, Kistler DJ, Cohn JF (2009) Development of perceptual expertise in emotion recognition. Cognition 110:242–247PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Prehn K, Kazzer P, Lischke A, Heinrichs M, Herpertz SC, Domes G (2013) Effects of intranasal oxytocin on pupil dilation indicate increased salience of socioaffective stimuli. Psychophysiology 50(6):528–537PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Quirin M, Kuhl J, Dusing R (2011) Oxytocin buffers cortisol responses to stress in individuals with impaired emotion regulation abilities. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36:898–904PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Riem MM, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Huffmeijer R, van Ijzendoorn MH (2013) Does intranasal oxytocin promote prosocial behavior to an excluded fellow player? A randomized-controlled trial with Cyberball. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38(8):1418–1425PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Ross HE, Young LJ (2009) Oxytocin and the neural mechanisms regulating social cognition and affiliative behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol 30:534–547PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Rutherford HJV, Lindell AK (2011) Thriving and surviving: approach and avoidance motivation and lateralization. Emot Rev 3(3):333–343CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Savaskan E, Ehrhardt R, Schulz A, Walter M, Schachinger H (2008) Post-learning intranasal oxytocin modulates human memory for facial identity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33(3):368–374PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Scheele D, Striepens N, Güntürkün O, Deutschländer S, Maier W, Kendrick KM, Hurlemann R (2012) Oxytocin modulates social distance between males and females. J Neurosci 32(46):16074–16079PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Scher CD, Murray BS, Asmundson GJG, McCreary DR, Forde DR (2001) The Childhood Trauma Questionnaire in a community sample: psychometric properties and normative data. J Trauma Stress 14(4):843–857PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Schulze L, Lischke A, Greif J, Herpertz SC, Heinrichs M, Domes G (2011) Oxytocin increases recognition of masked emotional faces. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36:1378–1382PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Shahrestani S, Kemp AH, Guastella AJ (2013) The impact of a single administration of intranasal oxytocin on the recognition of basic emotions in humans: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychopharmacology 38(10):1929–1936PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Shahrokh DK, Zhang TY, Diorio J, Gratton A, Meaney MJ (2010) Oxytocin–dopamine interactions mediate variations in maternal behavior in the rat. Endocrinology 151:2276–2286PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Steyer R, Schwenkmezger P, Notz P, Eid M (1997) Der Mehrdimensionale Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (MDBF). Hogrefe-Verlage für Psychologie, GöttingenGoogle Scholar
  73. Striepens N, Scheele D, Kendrick KM, Becker B, Schäfer L, Schwalba K, Reul J, Maier W, Hurlemann R (2012) Oxytocin facilitates protective responses to aversive social stimuli in males. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:18144–18149PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Tottenham N, Hare TA, Millner A, Gilhooly T, Zevin JD, Casey BJ (2011) Elevated amygdala response to faces following early deprivation. Dev Sci 14(2):190–204PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Van Ijzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ (2012) A sniff of trust: meta-analysis of the effects of intranasal oxytocin administration on face recognition, trust to in-group, and trust to out-group. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:438–443PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Van Ijzendoorn MH, Huffmeijer R, Alink LR, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Tops M (2011) The impact of Oxytocin administration on charitable donating is moderated by experiences of parental love-withdrawal. Front Psychol 2:258PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ (2003) Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nat Neurosci 6:624–631PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Weigand A, Feeser M, Gärtner M, Brandt E, Fan Y, Fuge P, Böker H, Bajbouj M, Grimm S (2013) Effects of intranasal oxytocin prior to encoding and retrieval on recognition memory. Psychopharmacology 227(2):321–329PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Winslow JT, Noble PL, Lyons CK, Sterk SM, Insel TR (2003) Rearing effects on cerebrospinal fluid oxytocin concentration and social buffering in rhesus monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology 28(5):910–918PubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Wittchen HU, Wunderlich U, Gruschwitz S, Zaudig M (1997) SKID-I. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. Achse I: Psychische Störungen. Hogrefe, GöttingenGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Melanie Feeser
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Yan Fan
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Anne Weigand
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Adam Hahn
    • 4
  • Matti Gärtner
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Sabine Aust
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Heinz Böker
    • 5
  • Malek Bajbouj
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Simone Grimm
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
    • 5
  1. 1.Cluster of Excellence “Languages of Emotion”Freie Universität BerlinBerlinGermany
  2. 2.Department of Psychiatry, Campus Benjamin FranklinCharité BerlinBerlinGermany
  3. 3.Dahlem Institute for Neuroimaging of EmotionFreie Universität BerlinBerlinGermany
  4. 4.Social Cognition Center CologneUniversity of CologneCologneGermany
  5. 5.Clinic for Affective Disorders and General PsychiatryPsychiatric University Hospital ZürichZurichSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations