Psychopharmacology

, Volume 181, Issue 1, pp 101–106 | Cite as

Reinforcing effects of caffeine and theobromine as found in chocolate

Original Investigation

Abstract

Rationale

Although in a previous study we showed that caffeine and theobromine were the main psychopharmacologically active constituents in a 50-g bar of chocolate, mere activity does not guarantee a role in our liking for the food.

Objectives

Our aim was to see if liking for a drink repeatedly paired with these amounts of caffeine and theobromine would increase compared to a placebo-paired drink.

Methods

Participants (n=64) consumed a ‘novel’ drink + treatment capsule on six non-consecutive mornings using a double-blind, placebo-controlled independent-sample design. Aspects of liking and intensity of various sensory descriptors for these drinks were measured at every drink collection. Treatment capsules contained either an ecologically relevant dose combination of 19-mg caffeine and 250-mg theobromine or a placebo.

Results

Liking for the drink paired with the methylxanthine-containing capsules increased over time compared to the placebo-paired drink. This highly significant effect was confirmed by subjective, retrospective changes in liking for the drink.

Conclusions

Methylxanthines in amounts found in 50-g chocolate may well contribute to our liking for chocolate, especially to the more acquired taste for dark chocolate.

Keywords

Chocolate Cocoa Caffeine Theobromine Flavour preference conditioning 

References

  1. Czok G (1974) Zur Frage der biologischen Wirksamkeit von Methylxanthinen in Kakaoprodukten. Z Ernährwiss 13:165–171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. De Houwer J, Thomas S, Baeyens F (2001) Associative learning of likes and dislikes: a review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychol Bull 127:853–869CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Desor JA, Maller O, Greene LS (1977) Preference for sweet in humans: infants, children, and adults. In: Weiffenbach JM (ed) Taste and development: the genesis of sweet preference. U.S. Dept. of Health, Education and Welfare, Bethesda, MD, USA, pp 161–172Google Scholar
  4. Di Marzo V, Sepe N, De Petrocellis L, Berger A, Crozier G, Fride E, Mechoulam R (1998) Trick or treat from food endocannabinoids? Nature 396:636CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Di Tomaso E, Beltramo M, Piomelli D (1996) Brain cannabinoids in chocolate. Nature 382(6593):677–678CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Drewnowski A, Greenwood MR (1983) Cream and sugar: human preferences for high-fat foods. Physiol Behav 30:629–633PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Drouillard DD, Vesell ES, Dvorchik BH (1978) Studies on theobromine disposition in normal subjects. Clin Pharmacol Ther 23:296–302PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Fredholm BB, Lindström K (1999) Autoradiographic comparison of the potency of several structurally unrelated adenosine receptor antagonists at adenosine A1 and A2A receptors. Eur J Pharmacol 380:197–202CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Gendall KA, Joyce PR, Sullivan PF (1997) Impact of definition on prevalence of food cravings in a random sample of young women. Appetite 28:63–72CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Gibson EL, Desmond E (1999) Chocolate craving and hunger state: implications for the acquisition and expression of appetite and food choice. Appetite 32:219–240CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Hamilton S (1992) Why the lady loves C6H5(CH2)2NH2. New Sci 132:26–28Google Scholar
  12. Hill AJ, Heaton-Brown L (1994) The experience of food craving; a prospective investigation in healthy women. J Psychosom Res 38:801–814CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. James JE (1991) Caffeine and health. Academic Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  14. James JE (1997) Understanding caffeine: a biobehavioral analysis. Sage Publications Ltd, LondonGoogle Scholar
  15. Levey AB, Martin I (1975) Classical conditioning of human “evaluation” responses. Behav Res Ther 13:221–226CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Liebowitz MR, Klein DF (1979) Hysteroid dysphoria. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2:555–575Google Scholar
  17. Lotshaw SC, Bradley JR, Brooks LR (1996) Illustrating caffeine’s pharmacological and expectancy effects utilizing a balanced placebo design. J Drug Educ 26:13–24PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. MAFF (1998) Survey of caffeine and other methylxanthines in energy drinks and other caffeine-containing products (updated). Food Surveillance Information Sheet 144Google Scholar
  19. Mela D (1992) The perception and acceptance of dietary fat: What, Who, Why? Tomorrow’s Nutr 17(Suppl 1):74–86Google Scholar
  20. Michener W, Rozin P (1994) Pharmacological versus sensory factors in the satiation of chocolate craving. Physiol Behav 56:419–422CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Miners JO, Attwood J, Birkett DJ (1982) Theobromine metabolism in man. Drug Metab Dispos 10:672–675PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Mumford GK, Evans SM, Kaminski BJ, Preston KL, Sannerud CA, Silverman K, Griffiths RR (1994) Discriminative stimulus and subjective effects of theobromine and caffeine in humans. Psychopharmacology 115:1–8PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Pavlov IP (1928) Lectures on conditioned reflexes. Lawrence and Wishart, LondonGoogle Scholar
  24. Persson CGA, Karlsson JA, Erjefalt I (1982) Differentiation between bronchodilation and universal adenosine antagonism among xanthine derivatives. Life Sci 30:2181–2189CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Pliner P, Rozin P, Cooper M, Woody G (1985) Role of specific postingestional effects and medicinal context in the acquisition of liking for tastes. Appetite 6:243–252PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Richardson NJ, Rogers PJ, Elliman NA (1996) Conditioned flavour preferences reinforced by caffeine consumed after lunch. Physiol Behav 60:257–263CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Rodin J, Mancuso J, Granger J, Nelbach E (1991) Food cravings in relation to body mass index, restraint and estradiol levels: a repeated measures study in healthy women. Appetite 17:177–185CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Rogers PJ, Dernoncourt C (1998) Regular caffeine consumption: a balance of adverse and beneficial effects for mood and psychomotor performance. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 59:1039–1045CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Rogers PJ, Smit HJ (2000) Food craving and food “addiction”: a critical review of the evidence from a biopsychosocial perspective. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 66:3–14CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Rogers PJ, Richardson NJ, Elliman NA (1995) Overnight caffeine abstinence and negative reinforcement of preference for caffeine-containing drinks. Psychopharmacology 120:457–462PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Rogers PJ, Martin J, Smith C, Heatherley SV, Smit HJ (2003) Absence of reinforcing, mood and psychomotor performance effects of caffeine in habitual non-consumers of caffeine. Psychopharmacology 167:54–62PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Rozin P, Levine E, Stoess C (1991) Chocolate craving and liking. Appetite 17:199–212CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Rozin P, Wrzesniewski A, Byrnes D (1998) The elusiveness of evaluative conditioning. Learn Motiv 29:397–415CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Schlich P (1993) Uses of change-over designs and repeated measurements in sensory and consumer studies. Food Qual Prefer 4:223–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Scholey AB, Chandler C, Wesnes K (2000) Interactions between actual and informed caffeine content on subsequent cognitive performance. Proc Br Psychol Soc 8:45Google Scholar
  36. Smit HJ, Blackburn RJ (2004) The role of methylxanthines in the liking for chocolate. Appetite 43:119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Smit HJ, Rogers PJ (2000) Effects of low doses of caffeine on cognitive performance, mood and thirst in low and higher caffeine consumers. Psychopharmacology 152:167–173CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Smit HJ, Rogers PJ (2001) Potentially psychoactive constituents of cocoa-containing products. In: Hetherington MM (ed) Food cravings and addiction. Leatherhead Food RA Publishing, Leatherhead, pp 325–349Google Scholar
  39. Smit HJ, Rogers PJ (2002a) Effects of ‘energy’ drinks on mood and mental performance: critical methodology. Food Qual Prefer 13:317–326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Smit HJ, Rogers PJ (2002b) Effects of caffeine on mood. Pharmacopsychoecologia 15:231–258Google Scholar
  41. Smit HJ, Gaffan EA, Rogers PJ (2004) Methylxanthines are the psycho-pharmacologically active constituents of chocolate. Psychopharmacology 176:412–419CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Stavric B (1988) Methylxanthines: toxicity to humans. 3. Theobromine, paraxanthine and the combined effects of methylxanthines. Food Chem Toxicol 26:725–733CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Steiner JE (1977) Facial expressions of the neonate infant indicating the hedonics of food-related chemical stimuli. In: Weiffenbach JM (ed) Taste and development: the genesis of sweet preference. U.S. Dept. of Health, Education and Welfare, Bethesda, MD, USA, pp 173–189Google Scholar
  44. Tarka SM Jr, Arnaud MJ, Dvorchik BH, Vesell ES (1983) Theobromine kinetics and metabolic disposition. Clin Pharmacol Ther 34:546–555PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Yeomans MR, Spetch H, Rogers PJ (1998) Conditioned flavour preference negatively reinforced by caffeine in human volunteers. Psychopharmacology 137:401–409CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Yeomans MR, Jackson A, Lee MD, Steer B, Tinley E, Durlach P, Rogers PJ (2000) Acquisition and extinction of flavour preferences conditioned by caffeine in humans. Appetite 35:131–141CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of BristolBristolUK

Personalised recommendations