Advertisement

Psychopharmacology

, Volume 185, Issue 2, pp 169–178 | Cite as

Alcohol attentional bias: drinking salience or cognitive impairment?

  • Javad Salehi Fadardi
  • W. Miles CoxEmail author
Original Investigation

Abstract

Objective

This study evaluated whether alcohol attentional bias is an artifact of excessive drinkers’ impaired cognitive functioning, which adversely affects their performance on the classic Stroop test (a measure of inhibitory control) and the Shipley Institute of Living Scale (SILS; a measure of verbal and abstraction ability). Both tests measure aspects of executive cognitive functioning (ECF).

Methods

Social drinkers (N=87) and alcohol-dependent drinkers (N=47) completed a measure of alcohol consumption, classic and alcohol-related Stroop tests, and the SILS.

Results

A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) showed that the dependent drinkers were poorer on the cognitive measures (SILS scores and classic Stroop interference) and had greater alcohol attentional bias than the social drinkers. An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) in which the cognitive measures were controlled showed that the dependent drinkers’ greater alcohol attentional bias was not an artifact of their poorer cognitive performance.

Conclusion

The results are discussed in terms of cognitive–motivational models, which suggest that excessive drinking sensitizes alcohol abusers’ attentional responsiveness to alcohol-related stimuli to a degree that exceeds the adverse effects of alcohol on their general cognitive functioning.

Keywords

Alcohol abuse Attentional bias Stroop paradigm Executive cognitive functioning Cognitive bias Current concern 

References

  1. Adleman NE, Menon V, Blasey CM, White CD, Warsofsky IS, Glover GH, Reiss AL (2002) A developmental fMRI study of the Stroop color–word task. Neuroimage 16(1):61–75CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Algom D, Chajut E, Lev S (2004) A rational look at the emotional Stroop phenomenon: a generic slowdown, not a Stroop effect. J Exp Psychol Gen 133(3):323–338CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Allman JM, Hakeem A, Erwin JM, Nimchinsky E, Hof P (2001) The anterior cingulate cortex. The evolution of an interface between emotion and cognition. Ann N Y Acad Sci 935:107–117PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Ardouin C, Pillon B, Peiffer E, Bejjani P, Limousin P, Damier P, Arnulf I, Benabid AL, Agid Y, Pollak P (1999) Bilateral subthalamic or pallidal stimulation for Parkinson’s disease affects neither memory nor executive functions: a consecutive series of 62 patients. Ann Neurol 46(2):217–223CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Baayen HR, Piepenbrock R, van Rijn H (1993) The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM). Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PAGoogle Scholar
  6. Bantick SJ, Wise RG, Ploghaus A, Clare S, Smith SM, Tracey I (2002) Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. Brain 125(Pt 2):310–319CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bauer D, Cox WM (1998) Alcohol related words are distracting to both alcohol abusers and non-abusers in the Stroop colour-naming task. Addiction 93(10):1539–1542CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Blume AW, Marlatt GA, Schmaling KB (2000) Executive cognitive function and heavy drinking behavior among college students. Psychol Addict Behav 14(3):299–302CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Brown M, Besner D (2001) On a variant of Stroop’s paradigm: which cognitions press your buttons? Mem Cognit 29(6):903–904PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Brown SA, Tapert SF, Granholm E, Delis DC (2000) Neurocognitive functioning of adolescents: effects of protracted alcohol use. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 24:164–171CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Bunge SA, Klingberg T, Jacobsen RB, Gabrieli JD (2000) A resource model of the neural basis of executive working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97(7):3573–3578PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bush G, Frazier JA, Rauch SL, Seidman LJ, Whalen PJ, Jenike MA, Rosen BR, Biederman J (1999) Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the Counting Stroop. Biol Psychiatry 45(12):1542–1552CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI (2000) Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 4(6):215–222CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Cabeza R, Nyberg L (2000) Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J Cogn Neurosci 12(1):1–47PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Carter CS, Macdonald AM, Botvinick M, Ross LL, Stenger A, Noll D, Cohen JD (2000) Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97(4):1944–1948PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cedrus Corporation (1999) SuperLab Pro SKD (version 2.1) [software]. Cedrus Corporation, San Pedro, CA, USAGoogle Scholar
  17. Childress AR, Mozley PD, McElgin W, Fitzgerald J, Reivich M, O’Brien CP (1999) Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. Am J Psychiatry 156(1):11–18PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Ciesielski KT, Waldorf AV, Jung RE (1995) Anterior brain deficits in chronic alcoholism—cause or effect? J Nerv Ment Dis 183(12):756–761PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Cohen J (1992) A power primer. Psychol Bull 112(1):155–159CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Connor PD, Sampson PD, Bookstein FL, Barr HM, Streissguth AP (2000) Direct and indirect effects of prenatal alcohol damage on executive function. Dev Neuropsychol 18(3):331–354CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Cox WM (2000) Alcohol Use Questionnaire. Unpublished questionnaire. University of Wales, BangorGoogle Scholar
  22. Cox WM, Klinger E (1988) A motivational model of alcohol use. J Abnorm Psychol 97:168–180CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Cox WM, Klinger E (1990) Incentive motivation, affective change, and alcohol use: a model. In: Cox WM (ed) Why people drink: parameters of alcohol as a reinforcer. Gardner, New York, pp 291–314Google Scholar
  24. Cox WM, Klinger E (2004) A motivational model of alcohol use: Determinants of use and change. In: Cox WM, Klinger E (eds) Handbook of motivational counseling: concepts, approaches, and assessment. Wiley, Chichester, UK, pp 121–138Google Scholar
  25. Cox WM, Fadardi JS, Pothos EM (2005) The alcohol-Stroop test: theoretical considerations and procedural recommendations. Psychol Bull (in press)Google Scholar
  26. Crombez G, Hermans D, Adriaensen H (2000) The emotional Stroop task and chronic pain: what is threatening for chronic pain sufferers? Eur J Pain 4(1):37–44CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Dao-Castellana MH, Samson Y, Legault F, Martinot JL, Aubin HJ, Crouzel C, Feldman L, Barrucand D, Rancurel G, Feline A, Syrota A (1998) Frontal dysfunction in neurologically normal chronic alcoholic subjects: metabolic and neuropsychological findings. Psychol Med 28(5):1039–1048CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Davidson RJ (1998) Affective style and affective disorders: perspectives from affective neuroscience. Cogn Emot 12(3):307–330CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Davidson RJ, Irwin W (1999) The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends Cogn Sci 3(1):11–21CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Deckel AW, Bauer L, Hesselbrock V (1995) Anterior brain dysfunctioning as a risk factor in alcoholic behaviors. Addiction 90(10):1323–1334CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Derbyshire SW, Vogt BA, Jones AK (1998) Pain and Stroop interference tasks activate separate processing modules in anterior cingulate cortex. Exp Brain Res 118(1):52–60PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. de Ruiter C, Brosschot JF (1994) The emotional Stroop interference effect in anxiety: attentional bias or cognitive avoidance? Behav Res Ther 32(3):315–319CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Duka T, Townshend JM (2004) The priming effect of alcohol pre-load on attentional bias to alcohol-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 176(3–4):353–361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Fadda F, Rossetti Z (1998) Chronic ethanol consumption: from neuroadaptation to neurodegeneration. Prog Neurobiol 56:385–431CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Fuentes LJ (2001) Selective attention deficit in schizophrenia. Rev Neurol 32(4):387–391PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. George MS, Ketter TA, Parekh PI, Rosinsky N, Ring HA, Pazzaglia PJ, Marangell LB, Callahan AM, Post RM (1997) Blunted left cingulate activation in mood disorder subjects during a response interference task (the Stroop). J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 9(1):55–63PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. George MS, Anton RF, Bloomer C, Teneback C, Drobes DJ, Lorberbaum JP, Nahas Z, Vincent DJ (2001) Activation of prefrontal cortex and anterior thalamus in alcoholic subjects on exposure to alcohol-specific cues. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58(4):345–352CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Grant I, Mohns L (1975) Chronic cerebral effects of alcohol and drug abuse. Int J Addict 10(5):883–920PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Grant S, London ED, Newlin DB, Villemagne VL, Liu X, Contoreggi C, Phillips RL, Kimes AS, Margolin A (1996) Activation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93(21):12040–12045PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Green MW, Rogers PJ (1993) Selective attention to food and body shape words in dieters and restrained nondieters. Int J Eat Disord 14(4):515–517PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Heather N, Peters Timothy J, Stockwell T (2001) International handbook of alcohol dependence and problems. Wiley, ChichesterGoogle Scholar
  42. Hoaken PN, Giancola PR, Pihl RO (1998) Executive cognitive functions as mediators of alcohol-related aggression. Alcohol Alcohol 33(1):47–54PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Jones BT, Jones BC, Smith H, Copely N (2003) A flicker paradigm for inducing change blindness reveals alcohol and cannabis information processing biases in social users. Addiction 98:235–244CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Kilts CD, Schweitzer JB, Quinn CK, Gross RE, Faber TL, Muhammad F, Ely TD, Hoffman JM, Drexler KP (2001) Neural activity related to drug craving in cocaine addiction. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58(4):334–341CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Kinderman P (1994) Attentional bias, persecutory delusions and the self-concept. Br J Med Psychol 67(Pt 1):53–66PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Kindt M, Bierman D, Brosschot JF (1997) Cognitive bias in spider fear and control children: assessment of emotional interference by a card format and a single-trial format of the Stroop task. J Exp Child Psychol 66(2):163–179CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. King AC, Bernardy NC, Hauner K (2003) Stressful events, personality, and mood disturbance: Gender differences in alcoholics and problem drinkers. Addict Behav 28(1):171–187CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Klinger E (1975) Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychol Rev 82(1):1–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Klinger E (1977). Meaning and void: inner experience and the incentives in people’s lives. University of Minnesota Press, MinneapolisGoogle Scholar
  50. Klinger E (1987). Current concerns and disengagement from incentives. In: Halisch F, Kuhl J (eds) Motivation, attention, and volition. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, pp 337–347Google Scholar
  51. Klinger E (1996) The contents of thoughts: interference as the downside of adaptive normal mechanisms in thought flow. In: Sarason IG, Sarason BR, Pierce GR (eds) Cognitive interference: theories, methods, and findings. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 3–23Google Scholar
  52. Klinger E, Cox WM (2004) Motivation and the theory of current concerns. In: Cox WM, Klinger E (eds) Handbook of motivational counseling: concepts, approaches, and assessment. Wiley, Chichester, UK, pp 3–27Google Scholar
  53. Koob GF (2003) Alcoholism: allostasis and beyond. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 27(2):232–243PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Kucera H, Francis WN (1967) Computational analysis of present-day American English. Brown University Press, Providence, RIGoogle Scholar
  55. Lawton-Craddock A, Nixon SJ, Tivis R (2003) Cognitive efficiency in stimulant abusers with and without alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 27(3):457–464CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Lyvers M (2000) “Loss of control” in alcoholism and drug addiction: a neuroscientific interpretation. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 8(2):225–249CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. MacLeod CM (1991) Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull 109(2):163–203CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. MacLeod CM, MacDonald PA (2000) Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends Cogn Sci 4(10):383–391CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Mantere T, Tupala E, Hall H, Sarkioja T, Rasanen P, Bergstrom K, Callaway J, Tiihonen J (2002) Serotonin transporter distribution and density in the cerebral cortex of alcoholic and nonalcoholic comparison subjects: a whole-hemisphere autoradiography study. Am J Psychiatry 159(4):599–606CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Marksteiner J, Bodner T, Gurka P (2002) Alcohol-induced cognitive disorder: alcohol dementia. Wien Med Wochenschr 152(3–4):98–101CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Maylor EA, Rabbitt PM (1993) Alcohol, reaction time and memory: a meta-analysis. Br J Psychol 84(Pt 3):301–317PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. McCusker CG (2001) Cognitive biases and addiction: an evolution in theory and method. Addiction 96(1):47–56CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Mega MS, Cummings JL (1994) Frontal–subcortical circuits and neuropsychiatric disorders. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 6(4):358–370PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Mogg K, Bradley BP, Millar N, White J (1995) A follow-up study of cognitive bias in generalized anxiety disorder. Behav Res Ther 33:927–935CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Monahan JS (2001) Coloring single Stroop elements: reducing automaticity or slowing color processing? J Gen Psychol 128(1):98–112PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Moselhy HF, Georgiou G, Kahn A (2001) Frontal lobe changes in alcoholism: a review of the literature. Alcohol Alcohol 36(5):357–368PubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Nassauer KW, Halperin JM (2003) Dissociation of perceptual and motor inhibition processes through the use of novel computerized conflict tasks. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 9(1):25–30CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Nixon SJ, Parsons OA, Schaeffer KW, Hale RL (1995) A methodological study of the Shipley Institute of Living Scale in alcoholics and non-alcoholics: reliability, discriminating items and alternate forms. Appl Neuropsychol 2(3/4):155–160CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Noel X, Van der Linden M, Schmidt N, Sferrazza R, Hanak C, Le Bon O, De Mol J, Kornreich C, Pelc I, Verbanck P (2001) Supervisory attentional system in nonamnesic alcoholic men. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58(12):1152–1158CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Noel X, Sferrazza R, Van Der Linden M, Paternot J, Verhas M, Hanak C, Pelc I, Verbanck P (2002) Contribution of frontal cerebral blood flow measured by 99 mTc-bicisate SPECT and executive function deficits to predicting treatment outcome in alcohol-dependent patients. Alcohol Alcohol 37(4):347–354PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Pardo JV, Pardo PJ, Janer KW, Raichle ME (1990) The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 87(1):256–259PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Parker ES, Birnbaum IM, Boyd RA, Noble EP (1980) Neuropsychologic decrements as a function of alcohol intake in male students. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 4(3):330–334PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Paulus MP, Hozack N, Frank L, Brown GG (2002) Error rate and outcome predictability affect neural activation in prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate during decision-making. Neuroimage 15(4):836–846CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Pothos EM, Cox WM (2002) Cognitive bias for alcohol-related information in inferential processes. Drug Alcohol Depend 66(3):235–241CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Ravnkilde B, Videbech P, Rosenberg R, Gjedde A, Gade A (2002) Putative tests of frontal lobe function: a PET-study of brain activation during Stroop’s Test and verbal fluency. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 24(4):534–547PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Riemann BC, McNally RJ (1995) Cognitive processing of personally relevant information. Cogn Emot 9(4):325–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Roberts AJ, Koob GF (1997) The neurobiology of addiction: an overview. Alcohol Health Res World 21(2):101–106PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Robinson TE, Berridge KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev 18(3):247–291CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2003) Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol 54(1):25–53CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Salgado-Pineda P, Vendrell P, Bargallo N, Falcon C, Junque C (2002) Functional magnetic resonance in the evaluation of the activity of the anterior cingulate cortex using Stroop’s paradigm. Rev Neurol 34(7):607–611PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Selzer ML (1971) The Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test: the quest for a new diagnostic instrument. Am J Psychiatry 127(12):89–94Google Scholar
  82. Serra-Mestres J, Ring HA (2002) Evidence supporting a cognitive model of depression in Parkinson’s disease. J Nerv Ment Dis 190(6):407–410CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Sharma D, McKenna FP (2001) The role of time pressure on the emotional Stroop task. Br J Psychol 92(Pt 3):471–481CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Skutle A, Berg G (1987) Training in controlled drinking for early-stage problem drinkers. Br J Addict 82(5):493–501CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. Smith EE, Jonides J (1999) Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science 283(5408):1657–1661CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Spinks H, Dalgleish T (2001) Attentional processing and levels of symptomatology in Seasonal Affective Disorder (SAD): a preliminary longitudinal study. J Affect Disord 62(3):229–232CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Stacy AW (1997) Memory activation and expectancy as prospective predictors of alcohol and marijuana use. J Abnorm Psychol 106:61–73CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Steel C, Haworth EJ, Peters E, Hemsley DR, Sharma T, Gray JA, Pickering A, Gregory L, Simmons A, Bullmore ET, Williams SC (2001) Neuroimaging correlates of negative priming. Neuroreport 12(16):3619–3624PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Stetter F, Ackermann K, Bizer A, Straube ER, Mann K (1995) Effects of disease-related cues in alcoholic inpatients: results of a controlled “Alcohol Stroop” study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 19(3):593–599PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Stormark KM, Laberg JC, Nordby H, Hugdahl K (2000) Alcoholics’ selective attention to alcohol stimuli: automated processing? J Stud Alcohol 61(1):18–23PubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Stroop JR (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. J Exp Psychol 18:643–662CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Swick D, Jovanovic J (2002) Anterior cingulate cortex and the Stroop task: neuropsychological evidence for topographic specificity. Neuropsychologia 40(8):1240–1253CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Tiffany ST (1990) A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychol Rev 97(2):147–168CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. van Veen V, Cohen JD, Botvinick MM, Stenger VA, Carter CS (2001) Anterior cingulate cortex, conflict monitoring, and levels of processing. Neuroimage 14(6):1302–1308CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Vogel-Sprott M, Easdon C, Fillmore M, Finn P, Justus A (2001) Alcohol and behavioral control: cognitive and neural mechanisms. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 25(1):117–121CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Vogt BA, Finch DM, Olson CR (1992) Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: the anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. Cereb Cortex 2(6):435–443PubMedGoogle Scholar
  97. Waters H, Green MW (2003) A demonstration of attentional bias, using a novel dual task paradigm, towards clinically salient material in recovering alcohol abuse patients? Psychol Med 33(3):491–498CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Watts FN, McKenna FP, Sharrock R, Trezise L (1986) Colour naming of phobia-related words. Br J Psychol 77(Pt 1):97–108PubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Wehr A, Bauer LO (1999) Verbal ability predicts abstinence from drugs and alcohol in a residential treatment population. Psychol Rep 84(3 Pt 2):1354–1360PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Wells A, Matthews G (1999) The cognitive science of attention and emotion. In: Dalgleish T, Power MJ (eds) Handbook of cognition and emotion. Wiley, Chichester, UKGoogle Scholar
  101. Whalen PJ, Bush G, McNally RJ, Wilhelm S, McInerney SC, Jenike MA, Rauch SL (1998) The emotional counting Stroop paradigm: a functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division. Biol Psychiatry 44(12):1219–1228CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Wiers RW, van Woerden N, Smulders FT, de Jong PJ (2002) Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy and light drinkers. J Abnorm Psychol 111(4):648–658CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Wiers RW, Cox WM, Field M, Fadardi JW, Palfai T, Schoenmakers T et al (2005) The search for new ways to change implicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (in press)Google Scholar
  104. Williams JM, Mathews A, MacLeod C (1996) The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol Bull 120(1):3–24CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  105. Wright I, Waterman M, Prescott H, Murdoch-Eaton D (2003) A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development: typical developmental trends. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl 44(4):561–575CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Zachary RA (2000) Shipley Institute of Living Scale: revised manual. Western Psychological Services, Los AngelesGoogle Scholar
  107. Zack M, Belsito L, Scher R, Eissenberg T, Corrigall WA (2001) Effects of abstinence and smoking on information processing in adolescent smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 153(2):249–257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Zeigler DW, Wang CC, Yoast RA, Dickinson BD, McCaffree MA, Robinowitz CB, Sterling ML (2005) The neurocognitive effects of alcohol on adolescents and college students. Prev Med 40(1):23–32CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of PsychologyUniversity of Wales, BangorBangorUK
  2. 2.Ferdowsi University of MashhadMashhadIran

Personalised recommendations