Advertisement

Psychopharmacology

, Volume 171, Issue 3, pp 286–297 | Cite as

The effects of tyrosine depletion in normal healthy volunteers: implications for unipolar depression

  • Andrew McLean
  • Judy S. Rubinsztein
  • Trevor W. Robbins
  • Barbara J. SahakianEmail author
Original Investigation

Abstract

Rationale

In recent years, there has been a growing interest in the role of dopamine (DA) both in the pathogenesis of unipolar depression and in motivated behaviour. The innovative technique of acute tyrosine depletion presents an opportunity to characterise further its function in these domains.

Objective

The present study examined the physiological, subjective and cognitive effects of acute tyrosine depletion in healthy volunteers.

Methods

A double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel group design was employed. Half of the participants ingested a balanced amino-acid mixture (BAL) and the other half received an identical mixture except that tyrosine and phenylalanine were absent (TYR-free). Plasma amino acid concentrations and subjective ratings were monitored at both baseline (T0) and 5 h following consumption (T5) of the mixtures. A comprehensive neuropsychological test battery was also administered at T5.

Results

Relative to the BAL group, the reduction in TYR availability to the brain was more marked in the TYR-free group. Employment of psychological rating scales revealed that, compared with the BAL group, the TYR-free group became less content and more apathetic. For the affective go/no-go task, whilst the BAL group exhibited a happy latency bias, the TYR-free group demonstrated a sad latency bias. Furthermore, in the decision-making task, the rate at which the TYR-free group increased their bets in response to more likely outcomes was lower than that of the BAL group. Taken together, these neuropsychological findings strikingly paralleled those reported in previous investigations of unipolar depression. The experimental groups could not be differentiated on any of the other neuropsychological measures, including more classical assessments of fronto-executive function.

Conclusion

These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that dopaminergic factors are particularly involved in disrupted affect/reward-based processing characteristic of clinical depression.

Keywords

Tyrosine Depletion Dopamine Neuropsychology Reward Affective processing 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank the volunteers for participating and the staff of ward R4 for their assistance and patience. The study was funded by a Wellcome Trust Programme Grant awarded to Professors Trevor Robbins, Barry Everitt and Barbara Sahakian, and Dr. Angela Roberts, and was completed within the MRC Centre for Behavioural and Cognitive Neuroscience. A.M. was supported by a MRC Research Studentship and J.S.R. was the recipient of the Betty Behrens Research Fellowship at Clare Hall, Cambridge, and received a Sackler studentship. We thank Dr. Sarah McTavish for methodological advice and Dr. Mike Franklin and his team for performing the biochemical analyses of the blood samples.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Association, WashingtonGoogle Scholar
  2. Baker SC, Rogers RD, Owen AM, Frith CD, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RSJ, Robbins TW (1996) Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the tower of London task. Neuropsychologia 34:515–526PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Beauregard M, Chertkow H, Bub H, Murtha S, Dixon R, Evans A (1997) The neural substrate for concrete, abstract, and emotional word lexical: a positron emission tomography study. J Cogn Neurosci 9:441–461Google Scholar
  4. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW (1994) Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50:7–15PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR (1996) Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 6:215–225PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR (1997) Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 275:1293–1294PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Anderson SW (1998) Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 18:428–437PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J (1961) An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 4:561–571Google Scholar
  9. Beck AT, Steer RA, Garbin MG (1988) Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev 8:77–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Biggio G, Porceddu ML, Gessa GL (1976) Decrease of homovanillic, dihydroxyphenylacetic acid and cyclic adenosine-3',5'-monophosphate content in the rat caudate nucleus induced by the acute administration of an amino acid mixture lacking tyrosine and phenylalanine. J Neurochem 26:1253–1255PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Bond A, Lader M (1974) The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. Br J Med Psychol 47:211–218Google Scholar
  12. Bower GH (1981) Mood and memory. Am Psychol 36:129–148CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Browne S, Garavan J, Gervin M, Roe M, Larkin C, O'Callaghan E (1998) Quality of life in schizophrenia: insight and subjective response to neuroleptics. J Nerv Ment Dis 186:74–78CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Brozoski TJ, Brown RM, Rosvold HE, Goldman PS (1979) Cognitive deficit caused by regional depletion of dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey. Science 205:929–932PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Coull JT, Sahakian BJ, Middleton HC, Young AH, Park SB, McShane RH, Cowen PJ, Robbins TW (1995) Differential effects of clonidine, haloperidol, diazepam and tryptophan depletion on focused attention and attentional search. Psychopharmacology 121:222–230PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Delgado PL, Charney DS, Price LH, Aghajanian GK, Landis H, Heninger GR (1990) Serotonin function and the mechanism of antidepressant action. Reversal of antidepressant-induced remission by rapid depletion of plasma tryptophan. Arch Gen Psychiatry 47:411–418PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Ellenbogen MA, Young SN, Dean P, Palmour RM, Benkelfat C (1999) Acute tryptophan depletion in healthy young women with a family history of major affective disorder. Psychol Med 29:35–46CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Elliott R, Dolan RJ (1998) The medial prefrontal cortex in depression. In: Ebert D, Ebmeier KP (eds) New models for depression. Karger, Basel, pp 72–93Google Scholar
  19. Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, McKay AP, Herrod JJ, Robbins TW, Paykel ES (1996) Neuropsychological impairments in unipolar depression: the influence of perceived failure on subsequent performance. Psychol Med 26:975–989PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, Matthews K, Bannerjea A, Rimmer J, Robbins TW (1997) Effects of methylphenidate on spatial working memory and planning in healthy young adults. Psychopharmacology 131:196–206PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Elliott R, Rees G, Dolan RJ (1999) Ventromedial prefrontal cortex mediates guessing. Neuropsychologia 37:403–411CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Elliott R, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ (2000) Dissociable neural responses in human reward systems. J Neurosci 20:6159–6165PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Elliott R, Rubinsztein JS, Sahakian BJ, Dolan RJ (2002) The neural basis of mood-congruent processing biases in depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59:597–604CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Furst P, Pollack L, Graser TA, Godel H, Stehle P (1990) Appraisal of four pre-column derivatization methods for the high performance liquid chromatographic determination of free amino acids in biological materials. J Chromatogr 499:557–569PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Greenhouse SW, Geisser S (1959) On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika 24:95–112Google Scholar
  26. Harmer CJ, McTavish SF, Clark L, Goodwin GM, Cowen PJ (2001) Tyrosine depletion attenuates dopamine function in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology 154:105–111PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Howell DC (1997) Statistical methods for psychology, 4th edn. Duxbury Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  28. Huynh H, Feldt LS (1976) Estimation of the Box correction for degrees of freedom from sample data in the randomised block and split-plot designs. J Educ Stat 1:69–82Google Scholar
  29. Jacobs D, Silverstone T (1986) Dextroamphetamine-induced arousal in human subjects as a model for mania. Psychol Med 16:323–329PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Kapur S, Mann JJ (1992) Role of the dopaminergic system in depression. Biol Psychiatry 32:1–17PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Kimberg DY, D'Esposito M, Marah MJ (1997) Effects of bromocriptine on human subjects depends on working memory capacity. Neuroreport 8:3581–3585PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Koob GF, Nestler EJ (1997) The neurobiology of drug addiction. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 9:482–497PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Lapin IP, Oxenkrug GF (1969) Intensification of the central serotonergic processes as a possible determinant of the thymoleptic effect. Lancet 1:132–136PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Leyton M, Ghadirian AM, Young SN, Palmour RM, Blier P, Helmers KF, Benkelfat C (2000a) Depressive relapse following acute tryptophan depletion in patients with major depressive disorder. J Psychopharmacol 14:284–287PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Leyton M, Young SN, Pihl RO, Etezadi S, Lauze C, Blier P, Baker GB, Benkelfat C (2000b) Effects on mood of acute phenylalanine/tyrosine depletion in healthy women. Neuropsychopharmacology 22:52–63Google Scholar
  36. Luciana M, Collins PF (1997) Dopaminergic modulation of working memory for spatial but not object cues in normal volunteers. J Cogn Neurosci 9:330–347Google Scholar
  37. Luciana M, Depue RA, Arbisi P, Leon A (1992) Facilitation of working memory in humans by a D2 dopamine receptor agonist. J Cogn Neurosci 4:58–67Google Scholar
  38. Marin RS, Biedrzycki RC, Firinciogullari S (1991) Reliability and validity of the Apathy Evaluation Scale. Psychiatry Res 38:143–162CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. McAllister TW (2000) Apathy. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry 5:275–282CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF (1992) Manual for the profile of mood states. Educational and Industrial Testing Service, San DiegoGoogle Scholar
  41. McTavish SF, Callado L, Cowen PJ, Sharp T (1999a) Comparison of the effects of alpha-methyl-p-tyrosine and a tyrosine-free amino acid load on extracellular noradrenaline in the rat hippocampus in vivo. J Psychopharmacol 13:379–384Google Scholar
  42. McTavish SF, Cowen PJ, Sharp T (1999b) Effect of a tyrosine-free amino acid mixture on regional brain catecholamine synthesis and release. Psychopharmacology 141:182–188Google Scholar
  43. McTavish SF, McPherson MH, Sharp T, Cowen PJ (1999c) Attenuation of some subjective effects of amphetamine following tyrosine depletion. J Psychopharmacol 13:144–147Google Scholar
  44. McTavish SF, McPherson MH, Harmer CJ, Clark L, Sharp T, Goodwin GM, Cowen PJ (2001) Antidopaminergic effects of dietary tyrosine depletion in healthy subjects and patients with manic illness. Br J Psychiatry 179:356–360PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Mehta MA, Sahakian BJ, McKenna PJ, Robbins TW (1999) Systemic sulpiride in young adult volunteers simulates the profile of cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease. Psychopharmacology 46:162–174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Mehta MA, Swainson R, Ogilvie AD, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW (2001) Improved short-term spatial memory but impaired reversal learning following the dopamine D2 agonist bromocriptine in human volunteers. Psychopharmacology 159:10–20Google Scholar
  47. Muller U, von Cramon DY, Pollmann S (1998) D1- versus D2-receptor modulation of visuospatial working memory in humans. J Neurosci 18:2720–2728PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Murphy FC, Sahakian BJ, Rubinsztein JS, Michael A, Rogers RD, Robbins TW, Paykel ES (1999) Emotional bias and inhibitory control processes in mania and depression. Psychol Med 29:1307–1321CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Murphy FC, Rubinsztein JS, Michael A, Rogers RD, Robbins TW, Paykel ES, Sahakian BJ (2001) Decision-making cognition in mania and depression. Psychol Med 31:679–693CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Murphy FC, Smith KA, Cowen PJ, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ (2002) The effects of tryptophan depletion on cognitive and affective processing in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology 163:42–53CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Naranjo CA, Tremblay LK, Busto UE (2001) The role of the brain reward system in depression. Progr Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 25:781–823CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Nelson HE (1982) National adult reading test (NART): test manual. NFER-Nelson, WindsorGoogle Scholar
  53. Nie NH, Hadlai HC, Jenkins JG, Steinbrenner K, Bent DH (1970) Statistical package for the social sciences. McGraw-Hill, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  54. Oldendorf WH, Szabo J (1976) Amino acid assignment to one of three blood-brain barrier amino acid carriers. Am J Physiol 239:94–98Google Scholar
  55. Owen AM, Downes JJ, Sahakian BJ, Polkey CE, Robbins TW (1990) Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 28:1021–1034PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Owen AM, James M, Leigh PN, Summers BA, Marsden CD, Quinn NP, Lange KW, Robbins TW (1992) Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease. Brain 115:1727–1751PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Owen AM, Roberts AC, Hodges JR, Summers BA, Polkey CE, Robbins TW (1993) Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. Brain 116:1159–1175PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Hodges JR, Summers BA, Polkey CE, Robbins TW (1995) Dopamine-dependent frontostriatal planning deficits in early Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology 9:1–15Google Scholar
  59. Owen AM, Evans AC, Petrides M (1996) Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. Cereb Cortex 6:31–38PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Pandya DN, Yeterian EH (1996) Morphological correlations of human and monkey frontal lobe. In: Damasio AR, Damasio H, Christen Y (eds) Neurobiology of decision-making. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 13–45Google Scholar
  61. Pardridge WM (1977) Kinetics of competitive inhibition of neutral amino acid transport across the blood–brain barrier. J Neurochem 28:103–108Google Scholar
  62. Park SB, Coull JT, McShane RH, Young AH, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW, Cowen PJ (1994) Tryptophan depletion in normal volunteers produces selective impairments in learning and memory. Neuropharmacology 33:575–588PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Pluck GC, Brown RG (2002) Apathy in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 73:636–642CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Riedel WJ, Klaassen T, Deutz NE, van Someren A, van Praag HM (1999) Tryptophan depletion in normal volunteers produces selective impairment in memory consolidation. Psychopharmacology 141:362–369PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Robbins TW (2000) Chemical neuromodulation of fronto-executive functions in humans and other animals. Exp Brain Res 133:130–138PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Robbins TW, Everitt BJ (1996) Neurobehavioural mechanisms of reward and motivation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 6:228–236PubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Rogers RD, Everitt BJ, Baldacchino A, Blackshaw AJ, Swainson R, Wynne K, Baker NB, Hunter J, Carthy T, Booker E, London M, Deakin JF, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW (1999a) Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 20:322–339PubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Rogers RD, Owen AM, Middleton HC, Williams EJ, Pickard JD, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW (1999b) Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 20:9029–9038Google Scholar
  69. Rogers RD, Andrews TC, Grasby PM, Brooks DJ, Robbins TW (2000) Contrasting cortical and subcortical activations produced by attentional-set shifting and reversal learning in humans. J Cogn Neurosci 12:142–162PubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Rubinsztein JS, Rogers RD, Riedel WJ, Mehta MA, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ (2001) Acute dietary tryptophan depletion impairs maintenance of "affective set" and delayed visual recognition in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology 154:319–326PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Sahakian BJ, Morris RG, Evenden JL, Heald A, Levy R, Philpot M, Robbins TW (1988) A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson's disease. Brain 11:695-718Google Scholar
  72. Schildkraut JJ (1965) The catecholamine hypothesis of affective disorders: a review of supporting evidence. Am J Psychiatry 122:509–522PubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Schultz W (1997) Dopamine neurons and their role in reward mechanisms. Curr Opin Neurobiol 7:191–197PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Sheehan BD, Tharyan P, McTavish SFB, Campling GM, Cowen PJ (1996) Use of a dietary manipulation to deplete plasma tyrosine and phenylalanine in healthy subjects. J Psychopharmacol 10:231–234Google Scholar
  75. Smith KA, Fairburn CG, Cowen PJ (1997) Relapse of depression after rapid depletion of tryptophan. Lancet 349:915–919PubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Smith SE, Pihl RO, Young SN, Ervin FR (1987) A test of possible cognitive and environmental influences on the mood-lowering effect of tryptophan depletion in normal males. Psychopharmacology 91:451–457PubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Starkstein SE, Mayberg HS, Preziosi TJ, Andrezejewski P, Leiguarda R, Robinson RG (1992) Reliability, validity, and clinical correlates of apathy in Parkinson's disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 4:134–139PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Teasdale JD, Howard RJ, Cox SG, Ha Y, Brammer MJ, Williams SCR, Checkley SA (1999) Functional MRI study of the cognitive generation of affect. Am J Psychiatry 156:209–215PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Willner P (1983) Dopamine and depression: a review of recent evidence. Brain Res Rev 6:211–246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Wise RA (1989) The brain and reward. In: Liebman JM, Copper SJ (eds) Neuropharmacological basis of reward. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 377–424Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Andrew McLean
    • 1
  • Judy S. Rubinsztein
    • 1
  • Trevor W. Robbins
    • 2
  • Barbara J. Sahakian
    • 1
    Email author
  1. 1.Department of Psychiatry, University of CambridgeSchool of Clinical Medicine, Addenbrooke's HospitalCambridgeUK
  2. 2.Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUK

Personalised recommendations