Research in Engineering Design

, Volume 25, Issue 2, pp 109–124 | Cite as

Interpretation in design: modelling how the situation changes during design activity

Original Paper

Abstract

This paper presents a model of the way that designers move between situations when interpreting during design activity. Three hypotheses are presented that arise from this model: that designers change their situation during interpretation, that small changes in a source can lead to large changes in the representation and that changes to the situation have their origins in the experience of the designer. The paper demonstrates how this internal movement between situations can be computationally implemented using three examples. The systems implemented demonstrate the way that interpretation can lead to changes in the situation and present an example of how the changes to a designer’s situation can be guided by past experiences.

Keywords

Situated design Computational modelling Interpretation 

References

  1. Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptual Symbol Systems. Behav Brain Sci 22:577–660Google Scholar
  2. Barsalou LW (2005) Abstraction as dynamic interpretation in perceptual symbol systems. In: Gershkoff-Stowe L, Rakison D (eds) Building object categories, Carnegie Symposium Series. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp 389–431Google Scholar
  3. Barsalou LW (2007) Grounded Cognition. Annu Rev Psychol 59(1):617–645. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barsalou LW (2009) Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 364(1521):1281–1289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Beyer O, Cimiano P, Griffiths S (2012) Towards action representation within the framework of conceptual spaces: preliminary results. Paper presented at the workshops at the twenty-sixth AAAI conference on artificial intelligenceGoogle Scholar
  6. Bilda Z, Gero JS, Purcell T (2006) To sketch or not to sketch? That is the question. Des Stud 27(5):587–613CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carpenter GA, Grossberg S (1988) The ART of adaptive pattern recognition by a self-organizing neural network. Computer 21(3):77–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Carpenter GA, Grossberg S (1990) ART 3: hierarchical search using chemical transmitters in self-organizing pattern recognition architectures. Neural Netw 3(2):129–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chisholm RM (1982) The foundations of knowing. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, MNGoogle Scholar
  10. Clancey W (1997) Situated cognition: on human knowledge and computer representations. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  11. Clancey WJ (1999) Conceptual coordination: how the mind orders experience in time. Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJGoogle Scholar
  12. Cross N (1982) Designerly ways of knowing. Des Stud 3(4):221–227. doi:10.1016/0142-694x(82)90040-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dittenbach M, Merkl D, Rauber A (2000) The growing hierarchical self-organizing map. Paper presented at the neural networks, 2000. IJCNN 2000, proceedings of the IEEE-INNS-ENNS international joint conference onGoogle Scholar
  14. Fish J, Scrivener S (1990) Effects of environmental context on recognition memory in an unusual environment. Percept Mot Skills 63:1047–1050Google Scholar
  15. Fodor JA (1975) The language of thought. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  16. Gabora L, Rosch E, Aerts D (2008) Toward an Ecological Theory of Concepts. Ecol Psychol 20(1):84–116. doi:10.1080/10407410701766676 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gärdenfors P (2000) Conceptual spaces: the geometry of thought. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  18. Gero JS (1998) Conceptual designing as a sequence of situated acts. In: Smith I (ed) Artificial intelligence in structural engineering. Springer, Berlin, pp 165–177Google Scholar
  19. Gero JS, Fujii H (2000) A computational framework for concept formation in a situated design agent. Knowl-Based Syst 13(6):361–368CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gero JS, Kannengiesser U (2004) The situated function–behaviour–structure framework. Des Stud 25(4):373–391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gero JS, Smith G (2009) Context, situations and design agents. Knowl-Based Syst 22:600–609CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Goel AK (1997) Design, analogy, and creativity. IEEE Expert 12(3):62–70CrossRefMathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  23. Gombrich EH (1966) Norm and form: studies in the art of the renaissance. Phaidon, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  24. Graf P, Schacter DL (1985) Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 11(3):501–518. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.11.3.501 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gross M, Do EY-L (2000) Drawing on the back of an envelope: a framework for interacting with application programs by freehand drawing. Comput Graph 24(6):835–849CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Harnad S (1999) The symbol grounding problem. Physica D 42:335–346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hinton GE, Osindero S, Teh Y-W (2006) A fast learning algorithm for deep belief nets. Neural Comput 18(7):1527–1554CrossRefMATHMathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  28. Jupp J (2005) Diagrammatic reasoning in design: computational and cognitive studies in similarity assessment. The University of Sydney, SydneyGoogle Scholar
  29. Jupp J, Gero J (2010) Let’s Look at Style: Visual and Spatial Representation and Reasoning in Design. In: Argamon S, Burns K, Dubnov S (eds) The structure of style. Springer, Berlin, pp 159–195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kennedy PJ, Shapiro ML (2004) Retrieving Memories via Internal Context Requires the Hippocampus. J Neurosci 24(31):6979–6985. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1388-04.2004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kohonen T (1990) The self-organizing map. Proc IEEE 78(9):1464–1480. doi:10.1109/5.58325 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lee H, Grosse R, Ranganath R, Ng AY (2009) Convolutional deep belief networks for scalable unsupervised learning of hierarchical representations. Paper presented at the proceedings of the 26th annual international conference on machine learningGoogle Scholar
  33. Menezes A, Lawson B (2006) How designers perceive sketches. Des Stud 27(5):571–585CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Minsky M (1975) A framework for representing knowledge. In: Winston PH (ed) The psychology of computer vision. McGraw-Hill, New York, pp 211–277Google Scholar
  35. Montare A (1994) Knowledge acquired from learning: new evidence of hierarchical conceptualization. Percept Mot Skills 79(2):975–993. doi:10.2466/pms.1994.79.2.975 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Mountcastle V (1997) The columnar organization of the neocortex. Brain 120:701–722CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Murphy GL (2002) The big book of concepts. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  38. Newell A (1994) Unified theories of cognition, vol 187. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  39. Nosofsky RM (1988) Similarity, frequency, and category representations. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 14(1):54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Peng W, Gero J (2006) Concept formation in a design optimization tool. In: van Leeuwen JP, Timmermans HJP (eds) Innovations in design decision support systems in architecture and urban planning. Springer, Netherlands, pp 293–308Google Scholar
  41. Piaget J (1954) The construction of reality in the child. Basic Books, LondonCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Pylyshyn ZW (1977) What the mind’s eye tells the mind’s brain: a critique of mental imagery. Images Percept Knowl. Springer, Berlin, pp 1–36Google Scholar
  43. Pylyshyn ZW (1984) Computation and cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  44. Riecke L, van Opstal A, Goebel R, Formisano E (2007) Sensory-perceptual transformations in primary auditory cortex. J Neurosci 27(46):12684–12689CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rosch E (1978) Principles of Categorization. In: Rosch E, Lloyd BB (eds) Cognition and categorization. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, HillsdaleGoogle Scholar
  46. Schacter D (1987) Implicit memory: history and current status. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 13(3):501–518CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Schon D, Wiggins G (1992) Kinds of seeing and their functions in designing. Des Stud 13(2):135–156. doi:citeulike-article-id:8497732 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Shaffer D, Hatfield D, Svarovsky G, Nash P, Nulty A, Bagley E et al (2009) Epistemic Network Analysis: a prototype for 21st Century assessment of learning. Int J Learn Media 1(2):33–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Suwa M, Tversky B (1997) What do architects and students perceive in their design sketches? Des Stud 18(4):385–403CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Suwa M, Gero J, Purcell T (2000) Unexpected discoveries and S-invention of design requirements: important vehicles for a design process. Des Stud 21(6):539–567. doi:10.1016/s0142-694x(99)00034-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Thoreau HD (1851/1993) A year in Thoreau’s journal: 1851. Penguin Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  52. Tscherepanow M (2010) TopoART: a topology learning hierarchical ART network. Artif Neural Netw ICANN 2010. Springer, Berlin, pp 157–167Google Scholar
  53. Wright PC, Monk AF (1985) Evaluation for design. In: Sutcliffe A, Macualay L (eds) People and computers V. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 345–358Google Scholar
  54. Yaner PW, Goel AK (2008) Analogical recognition of shape and structure in design drawings. AI EDAM 22(2):117–128Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag London 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Australian Digital Futures InstituteUniversity of Southern QueenslandToowoombaAustralia
  2. 2.Krasnow Institute for Advanced Study and Computational Social ScienceGeorge Mason UniversityFairfaxUSA
  3. 3.Computer Science and ArchitectureUniversity of North Carolina at CharlotteCharlotteUSA

Personalised recommendations