Advertisement

AI & SOCIETY

, Volume 27, Issue 1, pp 111–127 | Cite as

Rhythmic synchrony and mediated interaction: towards a framework of rhythm in embodied interaction

  • Satinder P. GillEmail author
Original Article

Abstract

Our everyday interactions increasingly involve both embodied face-to-face communication and various forms of mediated and distributed communication such as email, skype, and facebook. In daily face-to-face communications, we are connected in rhythm and synchrony at multiple levels ranging from the moment-by-moment continuity of timed syllables to emergent body and vocal rhythms of pragmatic sense-making. Our human capacity to synchronize with each other may be essential for our survival as social beings. Moving our bodies and voices together in time embodies a potent pragmatic purpose that of being together. In this synchrony of self with other, witnessing and being present become part of each other. There is growing research into how rhythm and synchrony operate in embodied face-to-face interaction and this provides parameters for investigating the relations and differences in how we connect and are socially present in the embodied and distributed settings, and understanding the effect of one setting upon the other. This paper explores the arena of research into rhythm in human interaction, musical and linguistic, with a focus on the movements of body and voice. It draws together salient issues and ideas that would form the basis for a framework of rhythm in embodied interaction.

Keywords

Rhythm Interpersonal synchrony Entrainment Body moves Mutuality Sense-making Floating intentionality Social presence 

Notes

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank Tommi Himberg, Ian Cross, Caroline Nevejan, and Jane Liddel-King for their support and helpful comments in writing this paper.

References

  1. Anders S, Heinzle J, Weiskopf N, Ethofer T, Haynes J (2011) Flow of affective information between communicating brains. Neuroimage 54(1):439–446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arom S (1991) African polyphony and polyrhythm: musical structure and methodology. CUP, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bavelas JB (2007) Face-to-face dialogue as a micro-social context: the example of motor mimicry. In: Duncan SD, Cassell J, Levy ET (eds) Gesture and the dynamic dimension of language. Essays in honor of David McNeill. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 127–146Google Scholar
  4. Bavelas JB, Black A, Lemery CR, Mullett J (1987) Motor mimicry as primitive empathy. In: Eisenberg N, Strayer J (eds) Empathy and its development. CUP, Cambridge, pp 317–333Google Scholar
  5. Bavelas JB, Coates L, Johnson T (2000) Listeners as co-narrators. J Pers Soc Psychol 79:941–952CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bavelas JB, Gerwing J, Sutton C, Prevost D (2008) Gesturing on the telephone: independent effects of dialogue and visibility. J Mem Lang 58:495–520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Birdwhistle R (1970) Kinesics and context. University of Pennsylvania Press, PhiladelphiaGoogle Scholar
  8. Bispham J (2006) Rhythm in music. What is it? Who has it? And why? Music Percep 24(2):125–134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Blacking J (1973) How musical is man?. University of Washington Press, SeattleGoogle Scholar
  10. Buhusi C, Meck WH (2005) What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:755–765CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Carr L, Iacoboni M, Dubeau M, Mazziott J, Lenzi GL (2003) Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. In: proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the USA (PNAS) 100(9):5497–5502Google Scholar
  12. Clayton M, Sager R, Will U (2004) In time with the music: the concept of entrainment and its significance for ethnomusicology. ESEM Counterpoint 1:1–82Google Scholar
  13. Condon WS (1970) Radio interview, Boston University Radio available at: http://www.edu-cyberpg.com/Literacy/WhatresearchCondon.asp
  14. Condon WS, Ogston WD (1966) Sound film analysis of normal and pathological behavior patterns. J Nerv Mental Dis 143(4):338–347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Condon WS, Sander LW (1974) Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech. Integrated participation and language acquisition. Science 183:99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cross I (2006) Music and social being. Musicol Aust 28:114–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cross I (2008) Musicality and the human capacity for culture. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue: narrative in music and interaction, pp 147–167Google Scholar
  18. Cross I (2011) Music and biocultural evolution. In: Clayton M, Herbert T, Middleton R (eds) The cultural study of music: a critical introduction, 2nd edn. Routledge, LondonGoogle Scholar
  19. Cross I, Woodruff GE (2009) Music as a communicative medium. In: R Botha, C Knight (eds) The prehistory of language. Oxford, Oxford University Press, 1:113–144Google Scholar
  20. Duranti A (1997) Universal and culture-specific properties of greetings. Linguist Anthropol 7(1):63–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Efron D (1972) Gesture, race and culture. A tentative study of some of the spatio-temporal and ‘linguistic’ aspects of the gestural behavior of eastern Jews and southern Italians in New York City, living under similar as well as different environmental conditions. Mouton Press, The HagueGoogle Scholar
  22. Gill SP (2002) The parallel coordinated move: case of a conceptual drawing task. CKIR working papers, CKIR-1, ISBN 951-791-660-4Google Scholar
  23. Gill SP (2004) Body moves and tacit knowing. In: Gorayska B, Mey JL (eds) Cognition and technology: co-existence, convergence, and co-evolution. John Benjamin, Amsterdam, pp 241–266Google Scholar
  24. Gill SP (2007) Entrainment and musicality in the human system interface. AI Soc 21(4):567–600CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gill SP, Borchers J (2004) Knowledge in co-action: social intelligence in collaborative design activity. AI Soc 17(3–4):322–333Google Scholar
  26. Gill SP, Kawamori M (2002) Coordination of gestures in a non face-to-face setting. In: Rector M, Poggi I, Trigo N (eds) Gestures, meaning and use. Fundacao Fernando Pessoa, PortoGoogle Scholar
  27. Gill SP, Kawamori M, Katagiri Y, Shimojima A (2000) The role of body moves in dialogue. RASK 12:89–114Google Scholar
  28. Goffman E (1963) Behaviour in public spaces. The Free Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  29. Good D (1990) Repair and cooperation in conversation. In: Luff P, Gilbert GN, Froehlich D (eds) Computers and conversation. Academic Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  30. Grahn JA (2009) Neuroscientific investigations of musical rhythm: recent advances and future challenges. Contemp Music Rev 28(3):251–277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hall ET (1976) Beyond culture. Anchor Books, Random House, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  32. Hall ET (1983) The dance of life. The other dimension of time. Anchor Books, Doubleday, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  33. Hennenlotter A, Schroeder U, Erhard P, Castrop F, Haslinger B, Stoecker D, Lange KW, Ceballos-Baumann AO (2005) A common neural basis for receptive and expressive communication of pleasant facial affect. Neuroimage 26(2):581–591CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Himberg T (2008) Measuring co-operability in tapping dyads. In: K Miyazaki, Y Hiraga, M. Adachi, Y. Nakajima & M.Tsuzaki (eds) In: Proceedings of the 10th international conference on music perception and cognition ICMP10, pp 460–464Google Scholar
  35. Himberg T (2011) Interacting with responsive and non responsive tapping partners. In: 13th international rhythm perception and production workshop, Leipzig 13–15th July, p. 32Google Scholar
  36. Himberg T, Thompson M (2010) Dyadic entrainment and interaction in African dance. In: S Demorest, S Morrison, P Campbell (eds) Proceedings of the 11th International conference on music perception and cognition (CD-ROM) Seattle: University of Washington School of Music, pp 658–663Google Scholar
  37. Hove MJ, Risen JL (2009) It’s all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc Cognit 27(6):949–961CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ivry RB, Richardson TC (2002) Temporal control and coordination: the multiple timer model. Brain Cognit 48:117–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Ivry RB, Spencer RMC (2004) The neural representation of time. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:225–232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Iyer V (2002) Embodied mind, situated cognition, and expressive microtiming in African-American music. Music Percept 19(3):387–414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Jones MR (1976) Time, our lost dimension: toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychol Rev 83(5):323–355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Jones MR, Boltz M (1989) Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychol Rev 96:459–491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Kawamori M, Takeshi K, Akira S (1995) A Phonological study on Japanese discourse markers. In: proceedings of 9th spoken language processing workshop notes (SIG-SLP-9). Information Processing Society of Japan, pp 13–20Google Scholar
  44. Kawamori M, Takeshi K, Akira S (1998) Discourse markers in spontaneous dialogue. A corpus based study of Japanese and English. In: M Stede, L Wanner, E Hovy (eds) Proceedings of coling/ACL workshop on discourse relations and discourse markers, pp 93–99Google Scholar
  45. Kendon A (1972) Some relationships between body motion and speech. An analysis of an example. In: Siegman A, Pope B (eds) Studies of dyadic communication. Pergamon Press, New York, pp 177–210Google Scholar
  46. Kendon A (1983) Gesture and speech: how they interact. In: Wieman JM, Harrison RP (eds) Nonverbal interaction. Sage Publications, Berverly Hills, pp 13–45Google Scholar
  47. Kendon A (1990) Conducting interaction: patterns of behaviour in focused encounters. CUP, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  48. Kirschner S, Tomasello M (2009) Joint drumming: social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. J Exp Child Psychol 102:299–314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Kirschner S, Tomasello M (2010) Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolut Hum Behav 31:354–364CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Kita S, Ide S (2007) Nodding, aizuchi, and final particles in Japanese conversation: how conversation reflects the ideology of communication and social relationships. Pragmatics 39(7):1242–1254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Large EW (2000) On synchronizing movements to music. Hum Mov Sci 19:527–566CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Large EW (2008) Resonating to musical rhythm: theory and experiment. In: S Grondin (ed) psychology of time. Emerald, Bingley, pp 189–231Google Scholar
  53. Large EW, Jones MR (1999) The dynamics of attending: how people track time-varying events. Psychol Rev 106(1):119–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Large EW, Kolen JF (1994) Resonance and the perception of musical meter. Connect Sci 6(2&3):177–208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Large EW, Snyder JS (2009) Pulse and meter as neural resonance. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1169:46–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Leonard G (1981) The silent pulse. Bantam Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  57. Leslie K, Johnson-Frey SH, Grafton ST (2004) Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: toward a motor theory of empathy. Neuroimage 21:601–607CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Levinson SC (2011) Cross-cultural universals and communication structures. Unpublished manuscript prepared for the Ernst Strüngmann Forum, Language, music, and the brain: a mysterious relationship May 8–13Google Scholar
  59. Local J (2003) Variable domains and variable relevance: interpreting phonetic exponents. J Phonet 31:321–339CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Local J (2007) Phonetic detail and the organisation of talk-in-interaction. In: Proceedings of 16th international congress of phonetic sciences (ICPhS XVI), Saarbrücken, pp 6–10 AugustGoogle Scholar
  61. Local J, Walker G (2004) Abrupt-joins as a resource for the production of multi-unit, multi- action turns. Pragmatics 36(8):1375–1403CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. London J (2004) Hearing in time: psychological aspects of musical meter. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  63. Lucas G (2006) Music and time in Afro-Brazilian Congado rituals. translated version of experience and meaning in music performance url, Open University, of original paper in III Meeting of the Brazilian association of ethnomusicology, October 2006Google Scholar
  64. Macrae N, Duffy O, Miles L, Lawrence J (2008) A case of hand waving: action synchrony and person perception. Cognition 109(1):152–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Malinowski B (1923) The problem of meaning in primitive languages. In: Ogden CK, Richards IA (eds) The meaning of meaning. Routledge and Kegan Paul, London, pp 146–152Google Scholar
  66. Mallery G (1891) Greeting by gesture. D. Appleton and Company, New York. Reprinted from popular science monthly, February & MarchGoogle Scholar
  67. Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds) (2009) Musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  68. Marsh KL, Richardson MJ, Schmidt RC (2009) Social connection through joint action and interpersonal coordination. Topi Cognit Sci 1(2):320–339CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. McCauley J, Jones M, Johnston H, Miller N (2006) The time of our lives: life span development of timing and event tracking. Exp Psychol Genet 135:348–367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Merker B (2000) Synchronous chorusing and human origins. In: Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S (eds) The origins of music. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 271–300Google Scholar
  71. Meyer LB (1956) Emotion and meaning in music. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  72. Miall DS, Dissanayake E (2003) The poetics of babytalk. Hum Nat 14:337–364CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Miles L, Nind L, Macrae CN (2009) The rhythm of rapport: interpersonal synchrony and social perception. Exp Soc Psychol 45(3):585–589CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Nevejan C (2009) Presence and the design of trust. PhD Dissertation, University of AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  75. Nevejan C, Brazier F (in press) Granularity in reciprocity. AI & SocietyGoogle Scholar
  76. Patel A, Daniele J (2003) An empirical comparison of rhythm in language and music. Cognition 87:35–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Phillips-Silver J, Trainor LJ (2005) Feeling the beat: movement influences infant’s rhythm perception. Science 308(5727):1430CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Phillips-Silver J, Aktipis A, Bryant GA (2010) The ecology of entrainment: foundations of coordinated rhythmic movement. Music Percept 28(1):3–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Rabinowitch T, Cross I, Burnard P (2011) Between consciousnesses: embodied musical intersubjectivity. In: Reynolds D, Reason M (eds) Kinesthetic empathy in creative and cultural practices. Intellect Press (in press)Google Scholar
  80. Repp B (2005) Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of the tapping literature. Psychon Bull Rev 12:969–992CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Richardson DC, Dale R (2005) Looking to understand: the coupling between speaker’s and listener’s eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognit Sci 29:1045–1060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, Isenhower R, Goodman J, Schmidt RC (2007) Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Hum Mov Sci 26:867–891CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V (2001) Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:661–670CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Scheflen AE (1973) Communicational structure. Analysis of a psychotherapy transaction. Indiana University Press, BloomingtonGoogle Scholar
  85. Scheflen AE (1974) How behaviour means. Anchor Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  86. Shimojima A, Koiso H, Swerts M, Katagiri Y (1998) An informational analysis of echoic responses in dialogue. In: Proceedings of the 20th annual conference of the cognitive science society, pp 951–956Google Scholar
  87. Shockley K, Santana MV, Fowler CA (2003) Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. Hum Percept Perform 29(2):326–332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Shockley K, Baker AA, Richardson MJ, Fowler CA (2007) Articulatory constraints on interpersonal postural coordination. J Exp Psychol. Hum percept Performance 33(1):201–8Google Scholar
  89. Sperber D, Wilson D (1986) Relevance: communication and cognition. Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  90. Stephan K, Thaut M, Wunderlich G, Schicks W, Tian B, Tellmann SchmitzL, Herzog H, MacIntosh G, Seitz R, Homberg V (2002) Conscious and subconscious sensorimotor synchronization—prefrontal cortex and the influence of awareness. Neuroimage 15:345–352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Stern DN (1974) Mother and infant at play: the dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal, and gaze behaviours. In: M Lewis, LA Rosenblum (eds) The effect of the infant on its caregiver, pp 187–213Google Scholar
  92. Stobart H, Cross I (2000) The Andean Anacrusis: rhythmic structure and perception in Easter songs of Northern Potosí, Bolivia. British J Ethnomusicol 9(2):63–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Swaine J (2004) Entrainment and the concept of embodied rhythm cognition. Presentation at entrainment workshop, Open UniversityGoogle Scholar
  94. Thaut M (2005) Rhythm, Music and the brain. Scientific foundations and clinical applications, RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
  95. Toiviainen P, Luck G, Thompson M (2010) Embodied meter: hierarchical eigenmodes in music-induced movement. Music Percept 28(1):59–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Tolbert E (2001) The enigma of music, the voice of reason: music, language, and becoming human. New Lit Hist 32(3):451–465CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Travarthen C, Aitken KJ (1994) Brain development, infant communication, and empathy disorders: Intrinsic factors in child mental health. In Develop Psychopathol 16:597–633CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Trevarthen C, Daniel C (2005) Disorganized rhythm and synchrony: early signs of autism and Rett syndrome. Brain Dev 27:25–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Unkefer RF, Thaut MH (2002) Music therapy in the treatment of adults with mental disorders: theoretical bases and clinical interventions. MMB Music, St.LouisGoogle Scholar
  100. van der Gaag C, Minderaa RB, Keysers C (2007) Facial expressions: what the mirror neuron system can and cannot tell us. Soc Neurosci 2(3–4):179–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Vorberg D, Schulze HH (2002) Linear phase-correction in synchronization: predictions, parameter estimation, and simulations. J Math Psychol 46(1):56–87MathSciNetzbMATHCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Wilson M, Wilson TP (2005) An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking. Psychon Bull Rev 12:957–968CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Wing AM, Kristofferson AB (1973) The timing of interresponse intervals. Percept Psychophys 13(3):455–460CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Winkler I, Haden G, Ladinig O, Sziller I, Honing H (2009) Newborn infants detect the beat in music. In: Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the USA (PNAS) 106:2468–2471Google Scholar
  105. Woolhouse M, Tidhar D (2010) Group dancing leads to increased person-perception. In: Proceedings of the 11th international conference on music perception and cognition, Seattle, USA, pp 605–608Google Scholar
  106. Zentner M, Eerola T (2010) Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. In: Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the USA (PNAS). Online publication before print, p. 6Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag London Limited 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centre for Music and ScienceUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUK

Personalised recommendations