AI & SOCIETY

, Volume 23, Issue 4, pp 485–510 | Cite as

Does Japan really have robot mania? Comparing attitudes by implicit and explicit measures

  • Karl F. MacDorman
  • Sandosh K. Vasudevan
  • Chin-Chang Ho
Original article

Abstract

Japan has more robots than any other country with robots contributing to many areas of society, including manufacturing, healthcare, and entertainment. However, few studies have examined Japanese attitudes toward robots, and none has used implicit measures. This study compares attitudes among the faculty of a US and a Japanese university. Although the Japanese faculty reported many more experiences with robots, implicit measures indicated both faculties had more pleasant associations with humans. In addition, although the US faculty reported people were more threatening than robots, implicit measures indicated both faculties associated weapons more strongly with robots than with humans. Despite the media’s hype about Japan’s robot ‘craze,’ response similarities suggest factors other than attitude better explain robot adoption. These include differences in history and religion, personal and human identity, economic structure, professional specialization, and government policy. Japanese robotics offers a unique reference from which other nations may learn.

Notes

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for the helpful advice and kind assistance of Leslie Ashburn-Nardo, Mahzarin Banaji, Stephen J. Cowley, Anthony Faiola, Sara A. Hook, Hiroshi Ishiguro, Josette Jones, Peter H. Kahn, Sara Kiesler, Tatsuya Nomura, Satoshi V. Suzuki, Hiroaki Yamano, Hiroki Yokota, and three anonymous reviewers.

References

  1. Ashburn-Nardo L, Knowles ML, Monteith MJ (2003) Black Americans implicit racial associations and their implications for intergroup judgment. Soc Cogn 21:61–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Banaji MR (2001) Implicit attitudes can be measured. Roediger HL III, Nairne JS, Neath I, Surprenant A (eds) The nature of remembering: essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder. American Psychological Association Press, Washington, DC, pp 117–150Google Scholar
  3. Bargh JA, Chaiken S, Govender R, Pratto F (1992) The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. J Pers Soc Psychol 62:893–912CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barry B (2005) Better than people: why the Japanese want their robots to act more like humans. The Economist, December 24Google Scholar
  5. Bartneck C, Nomura T, Kanda T, Suzuki T, Kato K (2005) A cross-cultural study on attitude towards robots. In: Proceedings of the 11th international conference on human–computer interactionGoogle Scholar
  6. Bartneck C, Suzuki T, Kanda T, Nomura T (2007) The influence of people’s culture and prior experiences with Aibo on their attitude towards robots. AI Soc 21:217–230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Capek K (1921/2004) Rossum’s universal robots (R.U.R.). Penguin, LondonGoogle Scholar
  8. Castells M (2000) The rise of the network society, 2nd edn. Blackwell, MaldenGoogle Scholar
  9. Cooley M (2007) From judgment to calculation. AI Soc 21(4):395–409CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cowley SJ, MacDorman KF (2006) What baboons, babies, and Tetris players tell us about interaction: a biosocial view of norm-based social learning. Connect Sci 18(4):363–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dasgupta N, McGhee D, Greenwald A, Banaji M (2000) Automatic preference for white Americans: eliminating the familiarity explanation. J Exp Soc Psychol 36:316–328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Davis W (1996) The moral and political naturalism of Baron Kato Hiroyuki. Institute for East Asian Studies, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
  13. De Houwer J, Moors A (2007) How to define and examine the implicitness of implicit measures. In: Wittenbrink B, Schwarz N (eds) Implicit measures of attitudes: procedures and controversies. Guilford Press, New York, pp 179–194Google Scholar
  14. Dennett DC (1991) Consciousness explained. Little, Brown and Co., BostonGoogle Scholar
  15. Douglas M (1966) Purity and danger: an analysis of concepts of pollution and taboo. Routledge, LondonGoogle Scholar
  16. Engardio P (2005) Engineering: is the U.S. really falling? Businessweek, December 27Google Scholar
  17. Fazio RH, Olson MA (2003) Implicit measures in social cognition research: their meaning and use. Annu Rev Psychol 54:297–327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Feil-Seifer D, Skinner K, Mataric MJ (2007) Benchmarks for evaluating socially assistive robotics. Interact Stud 8(3):423–439Google Scholar
  19. Freud S (1919/2003) The uncanny [das Unheimliche] (D. McClintock, Trans.). Penguin, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  20. Greenwald AG, Banaji MR (1995) Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol Rev 102(1):4–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz LK (1998) Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol 74(6):1464–1480CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Greenwald AG, Nosek BA, Banaji MR (2003) Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. J Pers Soc Psychol 85:197–216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Harnad S (1989) Minds, machines, and Turing. J Exp Theor Artif Intell 1(1):5–25CrossRefMathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  24. Hausmann R, Tyson LD, Zahidi S (2007) The global gender gap report. World Economic Forum, GenevaGoogle Scholar
  25. Hornyak TN (2006) Loving the machine: the art and science of Japanese robotics. Kodansha, TokyoGoogle Scholar
  26. Houben K, Wiers RW (2007) Are drinkers implicitly positive about drinking alcohol? Personalizing the alcohol-IAT to reduce negative extrapersonal contamination. Alcohol Alcohol 42(4):301–307Google Scholar
  27. Ji L, Peng K, Nisbett RE (2000) Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. J Pers Soc Psychol 5:943–955Google Scholar
  28. Kagan RA (1994) Do lawyers cause adversarial legalism? A preliminary inquiry. Law Soc Inq 19(1):1–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kahn PH, Ishiguro H, Friedman B, Kanda T, Freier NG, Severson RL (2007) What is a human? Toward psychological benchmarks in the field of human-robot interaction. Interact Stud 8(3):363–390Google Scholar
  30. Karpinski A, Hilton JL (2001) Attitudes and the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol 1(5):774–788CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kincaid DL (1987) Communication East and West: points of departure. In: Kincaid DL (ed) Communication theory: Eastern and Western perspectives. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 331–340Google Scholar
  32. Komiya N (1999) A cultural study of the low crime rate in Japan. Br J Criminol 39(3):369–390CrossRefMathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  33. Lynn LH (2002) Engineers and engineering in the U.S. and Japan: a critical review of the literature and suggestions for a new research agenda. IEEE Trans Eng Manage 41(1):95–106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. MacDorman KF (2004) Extending the medium hypothesis: the Dennett–Mangan controversy and beyond. J Mind Behav 25(3):237–257Google Scholar
  35. MacDorman KF, Cowley SJ (2006) Long-term relationships as a benchmark for robot personhood. In: Proceedings of the 15th IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication. Hatfield, UK, September 6–9Google Scholar
  36. MacDorman KF, Ishiguro H (2006) The uncanny advantage of using androids in social and cognitive science research. Interact Stud 7(3):297–337CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Mazlish B (1993) The fourth discontinuity: the co-evolution of humans and machines. Yale University Press, New HavenGoogle Scholar
  38. Mori M (1970) Bukimi no tani (in Japanese). [The uncanny valley] (MacDorman KF, Minato T). Energy 7(4):33–35Google Scholar
  39. Mori M (1982) The Buddha in the robot: a robot engineer’s thoughts on science and religion. Tuttle, TokyoGoogle Scholar
  40. National Science Board (2006) Science and engineering indicators 2006, vol 1. National Science Foundation, ArlingtonGoogle Scholar
  41. Nomura T, Kanda T, Suzuki T, Kato K (2005) People’s assumptions of robots: investigation of their relationships with attitudes and emotions toward robots. In: Proceedings of the 14th IEEE international workshop on robot and human interactive communication, pp 125–130Google Scholar
  42. Nomura T, Kanda T, Suzuki T (2006) Experimental investigation into influence of negative attitudes toward robots on human–robot interaction. AI Soc 20(2):138–150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Nomura T, Tasaki T, Kanda T, Shiomi M, Ishiguro H, Hagita N (2007) Questionnaire-based social research on opinions of Japanese visitors for communication robots at an exhibition. AI Soc 21(1):167–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Nosek BA, Banaji MR (2001) The go/no-go association task. Soc Cogn 19(6):161–176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Olson MA, Fazio RH (2004) Reducing the influence of extra-personal associations on the implicit association test: personalizing the IAT. J Pers Soc Psychol 86(5):653–667CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Penke L, Eichstaedt J, Asendorpf JB (2006) Single attribute implicit association tests (SA-IAT) for the assessment of unipolar constructs: the case of sociosexuality. Exp Psychol 53(4):283–291Google Scholar
  47. Plato (360 BCE/1888) The republic, 3rd edn. In: Jowett B (Trans.). Clarendon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  48. Putnam H (1967) The mental life of some machines. In: Castaneda H (ed) Intentionality, minds and perception. Wayne State University Press, Detroit, pp 439–460Google Scholar
  49. Ramey CH (2005) The uncanny valley of similarities concerning abortion, baldness, heaps of sand, and humanlike robots. In: Proceedings of the views of the uncanny valley workshop, IEEE-RAS international conference on humanoid robots. Tsukuba, JapanGoogle Scholar
  50. Reubenfien E (1989) U.S. police walk different beat in Japan. The Asian Wall Street Journal, January 13–14Google Scholar
  51. Ross D, Dumouchel P (2004) Emotions as strategic signals. Rational Soc 16(3):251–286CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Sander RH, Williams ED (1989) Why are there so many lawyers? Perspectives on a turbulent market. Law Soc Inq 14(3):431–479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Scanlon C (2003) Japan considers jury system. BBC News, March 12Google Scholar
  54. Schodt FL (1988) Inside the robot kingdom: Japan, mechatronics, and the coming robotopia. Kodansha, TokyoGoogle Scholar
  55. Shibata T, Wada K, Tanie K (2004) Tabulation and analysis of questionnaire results of subjective evaluation of seal robot in Japan, UK, Sweden and Italy. In: Proceedings of the IEEE international conference on robotics and automation. New OrleansGoogle Scholar
  56. Singer CJ, Williams H, Adelmann B (1954) A history of technology. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  57. Solomon S, Greenberg J, Pyszczynski T (1998) Tales from the crypt: the role of death in life. Zygon J Relig Sci 33:9–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Turkle S (2007) Authenticity in the age of digital companions. Interact Stud 8(3):501–517Google Scholar
  59. Weil MM, Rosen LD (1995) A study of technological sophistication and technophobia in university students from 23 countries. Comput Hum Behav 11(1):95–133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Woelfel J (1987) Development of the Western model: toward a reconciliation of Eastern and Western perspectives. In: Kincaid DL (ed) Communication theory: Eastern and Western perspectives. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 299–314Google Scholar
  61. Xi Z (1967) In: Chu H, Lui T-C, Chan W-T (eds). Relections on things at hand: the neo-Confucian anthology. Columbia University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  62. Yamamoto S (1983) Naze nihonjin niha robotto arerugi ga nai no ka? (in Japanese) [Why aren’t Japanese allergic to robots?]. Gendai No Espuri 187:136–143Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag London Limited 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Karl F. MacDorman
    • 1
  • Sandosh K. Vasudevan
    • 1
  • Chin-Chang Ho
    • 1
  1. 1.School of InformaticsIndiana UniversityIndianapolisUSA

Personalised recommendations