Advertisement

Naturwissenschaften

, Volume 99, Issue 12, pp 1051–1062 | Cite as

Songs of two starling species: common traits versus adaptations to the social environment

  • C. Houdelier
  • M. Hausberger
  • A. J. F. K. Craig
Original Paper

Abstract

We analysed, for the first time, songs of the African Red-winged Starling Onychognathus morio and compared their general characteristics with those of the European Starling Sturnus vulgaris. Both species are gregarious during the non-breeding season, but European Starlings tend to nest in colonies, form unstable pair-bonds and are occasionally polygynous, whereas Red-winged Starlings form long-term pair-bonds and occupy exclusive nesting territories. Red-winged Starlings produced the same basic song categories as European Starlings: warbles and whistles. These two categories appeared to be involved in similar social interactions in the two species. However, several aspects of song behaviour differed between the two species: Red-winged Starlings, breeding in isolated nests, preferentially used whistles for long-distance communication and showed a simpler organization of warbling song. Whistles in the Red-winged Starling were mostly shared between birds and, in contrast to the European Starling, were not indicators of individual identity. Also in contrast to the European Starling, female song in Red-winged Starlings appeared very important throughout the breeding period. Our results suggest that some song characteristics in the two species are phylogenetically conserved whereas others are affected by the distinct social systems of the two species.

Keywords

Bird song Evolution of communication Starling 

References

  1. Adret-Hausberger M (1983) Variations dialectales des sifflements des étourneaux sédentaires en Bretagne. Z Tierpsychol 62:55–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1439–0310.1983.tb02141 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Adret-Hausberger M (1984) Seasonal variation in the whistles of starlings Sturnus vulgaris. Ibis 126:372–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Adret-Hausberger M (1986) Temporal dynamics of dialects in the whistled songs of starlings. Ethol 71:140–152. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00579 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Adret-Hausberger M (1988) Song differentiation and population structure: the example of whistled songs in an introduced population of European starlings Sturnus vulgaris in Australia. Ethol 79:104–115. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1988.tb00704 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Adret-Hausberger M (1989) The species repertoire of whistled song in the European starling: species-specific characteristics and variability. Bioacoustics 2:137–162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Adret-Hausberger M, Jenkins PF (1988) Complex organization of the warbling song in the European starling Sturnus vulgaris. Behav 107:138–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Adret-Hausberger M, Güttinger HR, Merkel FW (1990) Individual life history and song repertoire changes in a colony of starlings. Ethol 84:265–280CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Alström P (2001) The use of sounds in bird systematics. Introductory research essay no. 2. Uppsala/Dept Systematic Zoology, Evolutionary Biology Centre, Uppsala UniversityGoogle Scholar
  9. Baptista LF (1975) Song dialects and demes in sedentary populations of the white-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys nuttalli). Univ Calif Pub Zool 105:1–52Google Scholar
  10. Becker PH (1982) The coding of species-specific characteristics in bird sound. In: Miller EH, Kroodsma DE (eds) Acoustic communications in birds. Academic, New York, pp 213–252Google Scholar
  11. Beecher MD (1989) Signalling system for individual recognition: an information theory approach. Anim Behav 38:248–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Beletsky LD (1983) Aggressive and pair-bond maintenance songs of female red-winged Blackbirds Agelaius phoeniceus. Z Tierpsychol 62:47–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1983.tb02140 Google Scholar
  13. Bertin A, Hausberger M, Henry L, Richard-Yris MA (2007) Adult and peer influences on starling song development. Dev Psychobiol 49(4):362–74. doi: 10.1002/dev.20223 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bertram B (1970) The vocal behaviour of the Indian Hill Mynah Gracula religiosa. Anim Behav Monog 3:79–192Google Scholar
  15. Blumstein DT, Armitage KB (1997) Does sociality drive the evolution of communicative complexity? A comparative test with ground-dwelling sciurid alarm calls. Am Nat 150:179–200. doi: 10.1086/286062 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Boughey MJ, Thompson NS (1981) Song variety in the Brown Thrasher (Toxostoma rufum). Z Tierpsychol 56:47–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1981.tb01283 Google Scholar
  17. Bremond JC (1968) Recherches sur la sémantique et les éléments vecteurs d’information dans les signaux acoustiques du rouge-gorge Erithacus rubecula. Terre Vie 2:109–220Google Scholar
  18. Brooks RJ, Falls JB (1975) Individual recognition by song in white-crowned sparrows. III. Song features used in individual recognition. Can J Zool 53:1749–1761. doi: 10.1139/z75-101 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Brown ED, Farabaugh SM (1997) What birds with complex social relationships can tell us about vocal learning: vocal sharing in avian groups. In: Snowdon CT, Hausberger M (eds) Social influences on vocal development. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 98–127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Catchpole CK (1980) Sexual selection and the evolution of complex songs among common warblers of the genus Acrocephalus. Behaviour 74:149–166. doi: 0.1163/156853980X00366 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Catchpole CK, Komdeur J (1992) The song of the Seychelles Warbler Acrocephalus sechellensis an island endemic. Ibis 135:190–195. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1993.tb02831 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Catchpole CK, Slater PJB (2008) Bird song: biological themes and variations, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Craig AJFK, Feare CJ (2009) Family Sturnidae (Starlings). In: Del Hoyo J, Elliott A, Christie DA (eds) Handbook of the birds of the world, vol 14. Lynx Edicions, BarcelonaGoogle Scholar
  24. Craig AJFK, Hulley PE, Walter GH (1989) Nesting of sympatric red-winged and pale-winged starlings. Ostrich 60:69–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Dunbar RIM (2003) The origin and subsequent evolution of language. In: Christiansen MH, Kirby S (eds) Language evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 219–234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Eens M (1997) Understanding the complex song of the European Starling: an integrated ethological approach. Adv Stud Behav 26:355–434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Eens M, Pinxten R, Verheyen RF (1989) Temporal and sequential organization of song bouts in the European starling. Ardea 77:75–86Google Scholar
  28. Eens M, Pinxten R, Verheyen RF (1991) Male song as a cue for mate choice in the European starling. Behav 116:1141–1147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Eens M, Pinxten R, Verheyen RF (1993) Function of song and song repertoire in the European Starling Sturnus vulgaris: an avian experiment. Behav 125:51–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Falls JB, Krebs JR, McGregor PK (1982) Song matching in the great tit (Parus major): the effect of similarity and familiarity. Anim Behav 30(4): 997–1009. doi:10.1016/S0003–3472(82)80188 DOI:10.1016/S0003–3472%2882%2980188–7Google Scholar
  31. Feare CJ (1984) The starling. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  32. Feekes F (1982) Song mimesis within colonies of Cacicus cela: a colonial password? Z Tierpsychol 58:119–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Forrest TG (1994) From sender to receiver: propagation and environmental effects on acoustic signals. Am Zool 34:644–654Google Scholar
  34. Freeberg TM (2006) Social complexity can drive vocal complexity: group size influences vocal information in Carolina chickadees. Psychol Sci 17:557. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01743.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Gentner TQ, Hulse SH (1998) Perceptual mechanisms for individual vocal recognition in European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris. Anim Behav 56:579–594. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0810 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. George I, Cousillas H, Richard JP, Hausberger M (2008) A potential neural substrate for processing functional classes of complex acoustic signals. PLoS One 3(5):e2203. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002203 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Gill FB (1990) Ornithology. Freeman, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  38. Greene E (1999) Toward an evolutionary understanding of song diversity in oscines. Auk 116:373–386CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Hailman JP (1989) The organization of major vocalizations in the Paridae. Wilson Bull 101:305–343Google Scholar
  40. Hamao S, Ueda K (2000) Simplified song in an island population of the bush warbler Cettia diphone. J Ethol 18:53–57. doi: 10.1007/s101640070025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Hausberger M (1997) Social influences on song acquisition and sharing in the European Starling Sturnus vulgaris. In: Snowdon CT, Hausberger M (eds) Social influences on vocal development. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 129–156. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511758843.008 Google Scholar
  42. Hausberger M, Jenkins PF, Keene J (1991) Species-specificity and mimicry in bird song: are they paradoxes? A reevaluation of song mimicry in the European Starling. Behav 117:53–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Henry L, Hausberger M (2001) Differences in the social context of song production in captive male and female European starlings. CR Biol 324: 1167–1174. doi:10.1016/S0764–4469(01)01394–4 DOI:10.1016/S0764–4469%2801%2901394–4Google Scholar
  44. Henry L, Hausberger M, Jenkins PF (1994) The use of song repertoire changes with pairing status in male European starling. Bioacoustics 5:261–266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Henry L, Bourguet C, Coulon M, Aubry C, Hausberger M (2012) Sharing mates and nestboxes is associated with female ‘friendship’ in European starlings Sturnus vulgaris. J Comp Psychol. doi: 10.1037/a0029975
  46. Inman B (1986) Female vocalizations and their role in the avian breeding cycle. Ann NY Acad Sci 474:44–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1986.tb27997 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Jenkins PF (1978) Cultural transmission of song patterns and dialect development in a free-living bird population. Anim Behav 26(1):50–78. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(78), 900007-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Kreutzer M (1979) Etude du chant chez le bruant zizi Emberiza cirlus: le répertoire, caractéristiques et distribution. Behav 71:191–321CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Kroodsma DE (1976) Reproductive development in a female songbird: differential stimulation by quality of male song. Sci 192:574–575. doi: 10.1126/science.192.4239.574 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Kroodsma DE (1977) Correlates of song organisation among North American wrens. Am Nat 111:995–1008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Langmore NE (1998) Functions of duet and solo songs of female birds. Tree 13(4): 136–140. doi:10.1016/S0169–5347(97)01241 DOI:10.1016/S0169–5347%2897%2901241-XGoogle Scholar
  52. Maestripieri D (1999) Primate social organization, gestural repertoire size and communication dynamics: a comparative study of macaques. In: King BJ (ed) The origins of language: what non-human primates can tell us. School of American Research, Santa FeGoogle Scholar
  53. McCombe K, Semple S (2005) Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biol Let 1:381–385. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0366 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. McCracken KG, Sheldon FH (1997) Avian vocalizations and phylogenetic signal. Proc Natl Acad Sci 94:3833–3836PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Naas I (2009) Song variation and population identity in an African bird: Onychognathus morio, the Red-Winged Starling. Internship report Master 2, University of Rennes 1Google Scholar
  56. Nelson DA (1989) Song frequency as a cue for recognition of species and individuals in the field sparrow (Spizella pusilla). J Comp Psychol 103:171–176. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.103.2.171 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Pavlova D, Pinxten R, Eens M (2005) Female song in European starlings: sex differences, complexity, and composition. Condor 107(3):559–569. doi: 10.1650/0010-5422 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Pavlova D, Pinxten R, Eens M (2007a) Seasonal singing patterns and individual consistency in song activity in female European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Behav 144(6):663–680CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Pavlova D, Pinxten R, Darras VM, Eens M (2007b) Effects of nestboxes and males on female song activity in the European starling: an experimental study. Behav 144(10):1255–1271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Payne RB (1986) Bird songs and avian systematics. In: Johnston RF (ed) Current ornithology, vol 3. Plenum, New York, pp 87–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Pollard KA, Blumstein DT (2012) Evolving communicative complexity: insights from rodents and beyond. Phil Trans R Soc B 367(1597):1869–1878. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0221 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Powlesland RG (1983) Seasonal and diurnal variation in vocal behaviour of the South Island robin. New Zealand J Zool 10:225–232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Price JJ (2009) Evolution and life history correlates of female song in the New World blackbirds. Behav Ecol 20:967–977. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arp085 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Réthoré-Astalos B (2012) Functional importance of structural and sequential organisation in the Red-winged Starling song. Internship-report Master 2, University of Rennes 1Google Scholar
  65. Richard JP (1991) Sound analysis and synthesis using a microcomputer. Bioacoustics 3:45.60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Robinson FN (1975) Vocal mimicry and the evolution of bird song. Emu 75:23–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Rowan MK (1955) The breeding biology and behaviour of the Red-winged Starling Onychognathus morio. Ibis 97:663–705. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1955.tb01926 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Ryan MJ, Brenowitz EA (1985) The role of body size, phylogeny, and ambient noise in the evolution of bird song. Am Nat 126:87–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Sandell MI, Smith HG (1997) Female aggression in the European Starling during the breeding season. Anim Behav 53:13–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Searcy WA, Andersson M (1986) Sexual selection and song. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 17:507–533CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Slabbekoorn H, Smith TB (2002) Bird song, ecology and speciation. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 357:493–503. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.1056 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Slater PJB, Mann NI (2004) Why do the females of many birds species sing in the tropics? J Avian Biol 35:289–294. doi: 10.1111/j.0908-8857.2004.03392 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Sontag WA (1985a) Song and courtship of the Wattled Starling Creatophora cinerea. Malimbus 7:129–135Google Scholar
  74. Sontag WA (1985b) Zur Funktion des Gesangs bei Sturniden (Staren). Luscinia 45:181–200Google Scholar
  75. Sontag WA (1991) Habitatsunterschiede, Balzverhalten, Paarbildung und Paarbindung beim Lappenstar Creatophora cinerea. Acta Biol Benrodis 3:99–114Google Scholar
  76. Sorjonen J (1983) Transmission of the two most characteristic phrases of the song of the thrush nightingale Luscina luscina in different environmental conditions. Ornis Scand 14:278–288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Stoddard PK (1996) Vocal recognition of neighbours by territorial passerines. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, pp 356–374Google Scholar
  78. Vallet E, Beme I, Kreutzer M (1998) Two-note syllables in canary songs elicit high levels of sexual display. Anim Behav 55:291–297PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Wiley RH (2005) Individuality in songs of Acadian flycatchers and recognition of neighbours. Anim Behav 70:237–247. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.09.027 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Zuccon D, Cibois A, Pasquet E, Ericson PGP (2006) Nuclear and mitochondrial sequence data reveal the major lineages of starlings, mynas and related taxa. Mol Phylogenet Evol 41:333–344. doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2006.05.007 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • C. Houdelier
    • 1
  • M. Hausberger
    • 1
  • A. J. F. K. Craig
    • 2
  1. 1.UMR-CNRS 6552 Ethologie animale et humaineUniversité de Rennes 1RennesFrance
  2. 2.Department of Zoology and EntomologyRhodes UniversityGrahamstownSouth Africa

Personalised recommendations