Naturwissenschaften

, Volume 91, Issue 10, pp 489–492

The importance of crossroads in faecal marking behaviour of the wolves (Canis lupus)

  • Isabel Barja
  • Francisco Javier de Miguel
  • Felipe Bárcena
Short Communication

Abstract

For wolves (Canis lupus) scats play an important function in territorial marking behaviour. Depositing scats at strategic sites such as crossroads and on conspicuous substrates probably increases their effectiveness as visual and olfactory marks. It is therefore likely that scats will be deposited, and will accumulate, at particular crossroads where the probability of being detected by other wolves is greatest. To check this hypothesis, a wolf population in NW Spain was studied for two consecutive years, from May 1998 to March 2000, and the spatial distribution of 311 scats detected along roads (both at and away from crossroads) was analysed. This study was conducted over an area of 12,000 ha in Montes do Invernadeiro Natural Park. The results confirm that wolves preferably deposit their scats at crossroads (60.1%) and on conspicuous substrates (72.1%). Significantly more scats were found at intersections with numerous, easily passable roads connecting distant territories. Thus, wolves preferably deposit their faeces at crossroads with high accessibility and driveability. The larger the surface area of the crossroads, the more scats were found. Crossroads are therefore highly strategic points that facilitate the detection of scats.

References

  1. Alberts AC (1992) Constraints on the design of chemical communication systems in terrestrial vertebrates. Am Nat 139:562–569CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Asa CS, Mech LD, Seal US (1985) The use of urine, faeces and anal secretions in scent-marking by a captive wolf (Canis lupus) pack. Anim Behav 33:1034–1036Google Scholar
  3. Creel S, Fox JE, Hardy A, Sands J, Garrott B, Peterson RO (2002) Snowmobile activity and glucocorticoid stress responses in wolves and elk. Conserv Biol 16:809–814CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Fox MW, Cohen JA (1978) Canid communication. In: Sebeok TA (ed) How animals communicate. Indiana University Press, Indiana, USA, pp 728–748Google Scholar
  5. Kleiman DG (1966) Scent marking in the canidae. Symp Zool Soc Lond 18:167–177Google Scholar
  6. Macdonald DW (1980) Patterns of scent marking with urine and faeces amongst carnivore communities. Symp Zool Soc Lond 45:107–139Google Scholar
  7. Mech LD (1970) The wolf: ecology and behavior of an endangered species. Natural History Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  8. Peters RP, Mech LD (1975) Scent-marking in wolves. Am Sci 63:628–637PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Robinson IH, Delibes M (1988) The distribution of faeces by the Spanish lynx (Felis pardina). J Zool 216:577–582Google Scholar
  10. Rothman RJ, Mech LD (1979) Scent-marking in lone wolves and newly formed pairs. Anim Behav 27:750–760Google Scholar
  11. Vilà C, Urios V, Castroviejo J (1994) Use of faeces for marking in Iberian wolves (Canis lupus). Can J Zool 72:374–377Google Scholar
  12. Wayne RK, Vilà C (2003) Molecular genetic studies of wolves. In: Mech LD, Boitani L (eds) Wolves: behavior, ecology, and conservation. Chicago University Press, Chicago, USA, pp 218–238Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Isabel Barja
    • 1
  • Francisco Javier de Miguel
    • 1
  • Felipe Bárcena
    • 2
  1. 1.Departamento de BiologíaUniversidad Autónoma de MadridMadridSpain
  2. 2.Laboratorio de Parasitología, Facultad de FarmaciaUniversidad de Santiago de CompostelaSantiago de CompostelaSpain

Personalised recommendations