Advertisement

The Psychological Record

, Volume 59, Issue 3, pp 489–510 | Cite as

The Empathy Imbalance Hypothesis of Autism: A Theoretical Approach to Cognitive and Emotional Empathy in Autistic Development

Theoretical Article

Abstract

There has been a widely held belief that people with autism spectrum disorders lack empathy. This article examines the empathy imbalance hypothesis (EIH) of autism. According to this account, people with autism have a deficit of cognitive empathy but a surfeit of emotional empathy. The behavioral characteristics of autism might be generated by this imbalance and a susceptibility to empathic overarousal. The EIH builds on the theory of mind account and provides an alternative to the extreme-male-brain theory of autism. Empathy surfeit is a recurrent theme in autistic narratives, and empirical evidence for the EIH is growing. A modification of the pictorial emotional Stroop paradigm could facilitate an experimental test of the EIH.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. ARCHER, J. (1992). Ethology and human development. Hemel Hempstead, England: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
  2. ASHWIN, C., WHEELWRIGH T, S., & BARON-COHEN, S. (2006). Attention bias to faces in Asperger syndrome: A pictorial emotion Stroop study. Psychological Medicine, 36, 835–843.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. ATTWOOD, T. (1993). Why does Chris do that? Some suggestions regarding the cause and management of the unusual behaviour of children and adults with autism and Asperger Syndrome. London: National Autistic Society.Google Scholar
  4. BARON-COHEN, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  5. BARON-COHEN, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: A fifteen year review. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 3–20). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  6. BARON-COHEN, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. BARON-COHEN, S. (2003). The essential difference. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
  8. BARON-COHEN, S., RICHLER, J., BISA RYA, D., GURUNA THAN, N., & WHEELWRIGH T, S. (2004). The systemizing quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. In U. Frith & E. L. Hill (Eds.), Autism: Mind and brain (pp. 161–186). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  9. BARON-COHEN, S., RING, H. A., BULLMORE, E. T., WHEELWRIGHT, S., ASHW IN, C., & WILLIAMS, S. C. R. (2000). The amygdala theory of autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 355–364CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. BARON-COHEN, S., & WHEELWRIGH T, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163–175.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. BISCHOF-KOHLER, D. (1991). The development of empathy in infants. In M. Lamb & H. Keller (Eds.), Infant development: Perspectives from Germans-peaking countries (pp. 245–273). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  12. BLAIR, R. J. R. (1996). Brief report: Morality in the autistic child. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 571–579.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. BLAIR, R. J. R. (1999). Psychophysiological responsiveness to the distress of others in children with autism. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 477–485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. BLAIR, R. J. R. (2005). Responding to the emotions of others: Dissociating forms of empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 698–718.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. BLAIR, R. J. R., JONES, L., CLARK, F., & SMITH, M. (1997). The psychopathic individual: A lack of responsiveness to distress cues? Psychophysiology, 34, 192–198.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. BLAIR, J., SELLARS, C., STRICKLAND, I., CLARK, F., WILLIAMS, A., SMITH, M., Et al. (1996). Theory of mind in the psychopath. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 7, 15–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. BUITELAAR, J. K., VAN ENGELAND, H., DE KOGEL, K. H., DE VRIES, H., & VAN HOOFF, J. A. R. A. M. (1991). Differences in the structure of social behaviour of autistic children and non-autistic retarded controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 995–1015.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. CALDWELL, P. (2006). Finding you finding me: Using intensive interaction to get in touch with people whose severe learning disabilities are combined with autistic spectrum disorder. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  19. CAPPS, L., KASARI, C., YIRMIYA, N., & SIGMAN, M. (1993). Parental perception of emotional expressiveness in children with autism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 475–484.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. CA RON, M. J., MOTTRON, L., BERTHIAUM E, C., & DAWS ON, M. (2006). Cognitive mechanisms, specificity and neural underpinnings of visuospatial peaks in autism. Brain, 129, 1789–1802.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. CHA RMAN, T. (2004). Why is joint attention a pivotal skill in autism? In U. Frith & E. L. Hill (Eds.), Autism: Mind and brain (pp. 67–87). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  22. COLLE, L., BARON-COHEN, S., & HILL, J. (2007). Do children with autism have a theory of mind? A non-verbal test of autism vs. specific language impairment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 716–723.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. COOPER, J. E.. (1994). Pocket guide to the ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders. Edinburgh, Scotland: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
  24. DALTON, K. M., NACEWICZ, B. M., JOHNSTONE, T., SCHAEFER, H. S., GERNS-BACHER, M. A., GOLDSM ITH, H. H., Et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 519–526.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. DARWIN, C. (1965). The expression of the emotions in Man and animals. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1872)Google Scholar
  26. DAVIS, M. (1996). Empathy: A social psychological approach. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
  27. DECETY, J., & JACKS ON, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71–100.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. DECETY, J., & LAMM, C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. The Scientific World Journal, 6, 1146–1163.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. DEELEY, Q., DALY, E., SURGU LADZ E, S., TUNS TALL, N., MEZEY, G., BEER, D., Et al. (2006). Facial emotion processing in criminal psychopathy: Preliminary functional magnetic resonance imaging study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 189, 533–539.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. DE VIGNEMONT, F., & SINGER, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 435–441.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. EISENBERG, N. (2000). Empathy and sympathy. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., Pp. 677–691). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  32. EISENBERG, N., & STRAYER, J. (1987). Critical issues in the study of empathy. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 3–13). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  33. FRITH, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford, England: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  34. FRITH, U., & HILL, E. L. (Eds.). (2004). Autism: Mind and brain. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  35. GARNER, I., & HAMILTON, D. (2001). Evidence for central cohesion: Children with autism do experience visual illusions. In J. Richer & S. Coates (Eds.), Autism: The search for coherence (pp. 75–85). London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  36. GERNSBA CHER, M. A., & FRYM IARE, J. L. (2005). Does the autistic brain lack core modules? Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 9, 3–16.Google Scholar
  37. GRANT, C. M., BOUCHER, J., RIGGS, K. J., & GRAYS ON, A. (2005). Moral understanding in children with autism. Autism, 9, 317–331.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. HANSMAN-WIJNANDS, M. A., & HUMM ELEN, J. W. (2006). Differentiele diagnostiek van psychopathie en autismespectrumstoornissen bij volwassenen: Empathiegebrek als kernsymptoom [Differential diagnosis of psychopathy and autism spectrum disorders in adults: Empathic deficit as a core symptom]. Tijdschrift Voor Psychiatrie, 48, 627–636.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. HAPPE, F. (1994). Autism: An introduction to psychological theory. London: UCL Press.Google Scholar
  40. HASS ENS TAB, J., DZIOBEK, I., ROGERS, K., WOLF, O. T., & CONVIT, A. (2007). Knowing what others know, feeling what others feel: A controlled study of empathy in psychotherapists. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195, 277–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. HILL, E. L., & FRITH, U. (2004). Understanding autism: Insights from mind and brain. In U. Frith & E. L. Hill (Eds.), Autism: Mind and brain (pp. 1–20). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  42. HINDE, R. A. (1991). From animals to humans. In M. S. Dawkins, T. R. Halliday, & R. Dawkins (Eds.), The Tinbergen legacy (pp. 31–39). London: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
  43. HINDE, R. A. (1995). Konrad Lorenz (1903-89) and Nikolaas Tinbergen (1907-88). In R. Fuller (Ed.), Seven pioneers of psychology (pp. 75–105). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  44. HIRSTEIN, W., IVERSEN, P., & RAMACHANDRAN, V. S. (2001). Autonomic responses of autistic children to people and objects. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 268, 1883–1888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. HOBSON, R. P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  46. HOFFMAN, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. HUGHES, R. (2003). Running with Walker: A memoir. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  48. JABBI, M., SWA RT, M., & KEYS ERS, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex. NeuroImage, 34, 1744–1753.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. KASA RI, C., SIGMAN, M. D., BAUMGA RTNER, P., & STIPEK, D. J. (1993). Pride and mastery in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 353–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. KAUFMAN, B. N. (1994). Son rise: The miracle continues. Tiburon, CA: HJ Kramer.Google Scholar
  51. KLIN, A., JONES, W., SCHU LTZ, R., & VOLKMA R, F. (2004). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. In U. Frith & E. L. Hill (Eds.), Autism: Mind and brain (pp. 127–159). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  52. KRUUK, H. (2003). Niko’s nature: A life of Niko Tinbergen and his science of animal behaviour. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  53. KYLLIAINEN, A., & HIETAN EN, J. K. (2006). Skin conductance responses to another person’s gaze in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 517–525.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. LANDSMAN, F. (Producer). (2003, July 30). My family and autism [Television broadcast]. London: BBC 2.Google Scholar
  55. LEDOUX, J. E., & PHELPS, E. A. (2000). Emotional networks in the brain. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., Pp. 157–172). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  56. MAGNEE, M. J. C. M., DE GELDER, B., VAN ENGELAND, H., & KEMNER, C. (2007). Facial electromyographic responses to emotional information from faces and voices in individuals with pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 1122–1130.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. MARKRAM, H., RINALDI, T., & MARKRAM, K. (2007). The intense world syndrome: An alternative hypothesis for autism. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 1, 77–96.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  58. MCHUGH, L., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., & BARNES-HOLMES, D. (2004). Perspective-taking as relational responding: A developmental profile. The Psychological Record, 54, 115–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. MCIN TOSH, D. N., REICHMANN-DECKER, A., WINK IELMAN, P., & WILBA RGER, J. L. (2006). When the social mirror breaks: Deficits in automatic, but not voluntary, mimicry of emotional facial expressions in autism. Developmental Science, 9, 295–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. MCKEAN, T. A. (1994). Soon will come the light: A view from inside the autism puzzle. Arlington, TX: Future Horizons.Google Scholar
  61. MERGES, E. M. (2003). Emotion contagion in children with autism: The effect on affect and behavior [Abstract]. Dissertation Abstracts International, Section B, 64, 2930.Google Scholar
  62. MOODY, E. J., MCINTOSH, D. N., MANN, L. J., & WEISS ER, K. R. (2007). More than mere mimicry? The influence of emotion on rapid facial reactions to faces. Emotion, 7, 447–457.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. O’NEILL, J. L. (1999). Through the eyes of aliens: A book about autistic people. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  64. ONTARIO ADULT AUTISM RESEARCH AND SUPPORT NETWORK. (2007). News bulletin. Retrieved June 18, 2007, from http://www.ont-autism.uoguelph.ca/news-20060902.htmlGoogle Scholar
  65. OTTER, L., & MASEFIELD, E. (2001). Facilitated communication at the Chinnor Resource Unit: A journey. In J. Richer & S. Coates (Eds.), Autism: The search for coherence (pp. 152–175). London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  66. PERNER, J., & LANG, B. (2000). Theory of mind and executive function: Is there a developmental relationship? In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 150–181). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  67. PINKER, S., & BLOOM, P. (1992). Natural language and natural selection. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 451–493). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  68. PRESTON, S. D., & DE WAAL, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–20.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. RAJENDRAN, G., & MITCHELL, P. (2007). Cognitive theories of autism. Developmental Review, 27, 224–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. RAMACHANDRAN, V. S. (2003). The emerging mind. London: Profile Books.Google Scholar
  71. REHFELDT, R. A., DILLEN, J. E., ZIOMEK, M. M., & KOWALCHUK, R. K. (2007). Assessing relational learning deficits in perspective-taking in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. The Psychological Record, 57, 23–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. RICHELL, R. A., MITCHELL, D. G. V., NEWMAN, C., LEONA RD, A., BARON-COHEN, S., & BLAIR, R. J. R. (2003). Theory of mind and psychopathy: Can psychopathic individuals read the “language of the eyes”? Neuropsychologia, 41, 523–526.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. RICHER, J. (1976). The social-avoidance behaviour of autistic children. Animal Behaviour, 24, 898–906.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. RICHER, J. (2001a). An ethological approach to autism: From evolutionary perspectives to treatment. In J. Richer & S. Coates (Eds.), Autism: The search for coherence (pp. 22–35). London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  75. RICHER, J. (2001b). The insufficient integration of self and other in autism: Evolutionary and developmental perspectives. In J. Richer & S. Coates (Eds.), Autism: The search for coherence (pp. 36–52). London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  76. ROBBINS, P., & JACK, A. I. (2006). The phenomenal stance. Philosophical Studies, 127, 59–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. ROGERS, J., VIDING, E., BLAIR R. J., FRITH, U., & HAPPE, F. (2006). Autism spectrum disorder and psychopathy: Shared cognitive underpinnings or double hit? Psychological Medicine, 36, 1789–1798.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. ROGERS, K., DZIOBEK, I., HASSENSTAB, J., WOLF, O. T., & CONVIT, A. (2007). Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 709–715.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. SAINSBURY, C. (n.d.). Holding therapy: An autistic perspective. Retrieved July 12, 2007, from http://www.nas.org.uk/nas/jsp/polopoly.jsp?d=364&a=2179
  80. SCAMBLER, D. J., HEPBURN, S., RUTHERFORD, M. D., WEHNER, E. A., & ROGERS, S. J. (2007). Emotional responsivity in children with autism, children with other developmental disabilities, and children with typical development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 553–563.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. SCOTTISH INTERCOLLEGIATE GUIDELINES NETWORK. (2007). Assessment, diagnosis and clinical interventions for children and young people with autism spectrum disorders. Edinburgh, Scotland: Author.Google Scholar
  82. SEIFERT, C. D. (1990). Theories of autism. Lanham, MD: University Press of America.Google Scholar
  83. SIGMAN, M., & CAPPS, L. (1997). Children with autism: A developmental perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  84. SIGMAN, M., DISSANAYAKE, C., CORONA, R., & ESPINOSA, M. (2003). Social and cardiac responses of young children with autism. Autism, 7, 205–216.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. SIGMAN, M. D., KASARI, C., KWON, J., & YIRMIYA, N. (1992). Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Development, 63, 796–807.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. SLOBODA, J. A., & JUSLIN, P. N. (2001). Psychological perspectives on music and emotion. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion: Theory and research (pp. 71–104). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  87. SMITH, A. (2006). Cognitive empathy and emotional empathy in human behavior and evolution. The Psychological Record, 56, 3–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. SONNBY-BORGSTROM, M. (2002). Automatic mimicry reactions as related to differences in emotional empathy. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 433–443.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. SONNBY-BORGSTROM, M., JONSSON P., & SVENSSON, O. (2003). Emotional empathy as related to mimicry reactions at different levels of information processing. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 3–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. TINBERGEN, N., & TINBERGEN, E. A. (1983). “Autistic” children: New hope for a cure. London: George Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
  91. WHEATLEY, T. (2005). My sad is all gone: A family’s triumph over violent autism. Lancaster, OH: Lucky Press.Google Scholar
  92. WHITEN, A. (1997). The Machiavellian mindreader. In A. Whiten & R. W. Byrne (Eds.), Machiavellian intelligence II: Extensions and evaluations(pp. 144–173). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. WILLIAMS, D. (1996). Autism: An inside-out approach. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  94. WILLIAMS, D. (1998). Autism and sensing: The unlost instinct. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  95. YIRMIYA, N., SIGMAN, M. D., KASARI, C., & MUNDY, P. (1992). Empathy and cognition in high-functioning children with autism. Child Development, 63, 150–160.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Dryburgh Industrial EstateDundeeUK

Personalised recommendations