Advertisement

The Psychological Record

, Volume 55, Issue 3, pp 439–460 | Cite as

Delay of Gratification and Delay Discounting: A Unifying Feedback Model of Delay-Related Impulsive Behavior

  • Brady ReynoldsEmail author
  • Ryan Schiffbauer
Article

Abstract

Delay of Gratification (DG) and Delay Discounting (DD) represent two indices of impulsive behavior often treated as though they represent equivalent or the same underlying processes. However, there are key differences between DG and DD procedures, and between certain research findings with each procedure, that suggest they are not equivalent. In the current article, evidence is presented to support the argument that DG and DD measure discrete, yet related, processes involved in delay-related impulsive behavior. Also presented is a theoretical “feedback model” for the relation between DG and DD. In the model, it is proposed that the processes measured by DG are less cognitive and less learning-mediated than those measured by DD. However, as proposed, ability to sustain choices for delayed rewards (DG) is still represented in the choice processes measured by DD through an individual’s learning history with DG types of situations; that is, the less a person is able to sustain choices for delayed rewards the more likely he or she will be to choose immediate rewards when given choices between larger delayed and smaller but more immediate options. The proposed model is consonant with observed consistencies and differences between DG and DD measures. From the proposed model, new research questions arise that would be lost in a continued conceptualization of DG and DD as equivalent measures.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. ALLEN, T., MOELLER, G., RHOADES, H., & CHEREK, D. (1998). Impulsivity and a history of drug dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 50, 137–145.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. ALLPORT, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & WINSTON.Google Scholar
  3. AYDUK, O., MENDOZA-DENTON, R., MISCHEL, W., DOWNEY, G., PEAKE, P., & RODRIGUEZ, M. (2000). Regulating the interpersonal self: Strategic self-regulation for coping with rejection sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 776–792.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. BICKEL, W., ODUM, A., & MADDEN, G. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: Delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146, 447–454.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. CARDINAL, R. N., PENNICOTT, D. R., SUGATHAPALA, C. L., ROBBINS, T. W., & EVERITT, B. J. (2001). Impulsive choice induced in rats by lesions of the nucleus accumbens core. Science, 292, 2499–2501.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. CHAPMAN, M. (1988). Constructive evolution: Origins and development of Piaget’s thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  7. COFFEY, S. F., GUDLESKI, G. D., SALADIN, M. E., & BRADY, K. T. (2003). Impulsivity and rapid discounting of delayed hypothetical rewards in cocaine-dependent individuals. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11, 18–25.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. CREAN, J. P., DE WIT, H., & RICHARDS, J. B. (2000). Reward discounting as a measure of impulsive behavior in a psychiatric outpatient population. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 8, 155–162.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. EVENDEN, J. L., & RYAN, C. N. (1996). The pharmacology of impulsive behavior in rats: The effects of drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement. Psychopharmacology, 128, 161–170.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. GRANT, H., & DWECK, C. S. (1999). A goal analysis of personality and personality coherence. In D. Cervone & Y Shoda (Eds.), The coherence of personality: Social-cognitive basis of consistency, variability, and organization (pp. 345–371). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  11. GREEN, L., FRY, A., & MYERSON, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: A life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 33–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. HINSON, J. M., JAMESON, T. L., & WHITNEY, P. (2003). Impulsive decision making and working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 29, 298–306Google Scholar
  13. JOHNSON, M. W., & BICKEL, W. (2002). Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 77, 129–146.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. LUTZ, D. J., & STERNBERG, R. J. (1999). Cognitive development. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (pp. 275–311). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  15. MADDEN, G., BICKEL, W., & JACOBS, E. A. (1999). Discounting of delayed rewards in opioid-dependent outpatients: Exponential and hyperbolic discounting functions? Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 7, 284–293.Google Scholar
  16. MADDEN, G., PETRY, N., BADGER, G., & BICKEL, W. (1997). Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: Drug and monetary rewards. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 5, 256–262.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. MAZUR, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, & H. Rachiin (Eds.), Quantitative analysis of behavior: Vol 5. The effects of delay and intervening events on reinforcement value (pp. 55–73). Hillsdale, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
  18. METCALFE, J., & MISCHEL, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. MISCHEL, H. N., & MISCHEL, W. (1983). The development of children’s knowledge of self-control strategies. Child Development, 54, 603–619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. MISCHEL, W. (1966). Theory and research on antecedents of self-imposed delay of reward. In B. A. Maher (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality research. (Vol. 3). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  21. MISCHEL, W. (1990). Personality dispositions revisited and revisited: A view after three decades. In L. A. Previn (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 111–134). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
  22. MISCHEL, W., & BAKER, N. (1975). Cognitive appraisals and transformations in delay behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 254–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. MISCHEL, W, CANTOR, N., & FELDMAN, S. (1996). Principals of self-regulation: The nature of willpower and self-control. In E. T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 329–360). New York: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
  24. MISCHEL, W., & EBBESEN, E. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 329–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. MISCHEL, W., EBBESEN, E., & ZEISS, A. R. (1972). Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 204–218.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. MISCHEL, W., & METZNER, R. (1962). Preference for delayed reward as a function of age, intelligence, and length of delay interval. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64, 425–431.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. MISCHEL, W., & MOORE, B. (1973). Effects of attention to symbolically presented rewards on self control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 172–179.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. MISCHEL, W., SHODA, Y., & PEAKE, P. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 687–696.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. MISCHEL, W., SHODA, Y., & RODRIGUEZ, M. (1989, May 26). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. MITCHELL, S. (1999). Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146, 455–464.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. MOBINI, S., CHIANG, T., HO, M., BRADSHAW, C. M., & SZABADI, E. (2000). Effect of central 5-hydoxytryptamine depletion on inter-temporal choice: A qualitative analysis. Psychopharmacology, 149, 313–318.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. ODUM, A. L., & RAINAUD, C. P. (2003). Discounting of delayed hypothetical money, alcohol, and food. Behavioural Processes, 64, 305–313.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. ORTNER, C. N. M., MACDONALD, T. K., & OLMSTEAD, M. O. (2003). Alcoholism intoxication reduces impulsivity in the delay-discounting paradigm. Alcohol & Alcoholism, 38, 151–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. OSTASZEWSKI, P., & KARZEL, K. (2002). Discounting of delayed and probabilistic losses of different amounts. European Psychologist, 7, 295-301.Google Scholar
  35. PEAKE, P., HEBL, M., & MISCHEL, W. (2002). Strategic attention deployment for delay of gratification in working and waiting situations. Developmental Psychology, 38, 313–326.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. PEARS, R., & BRYANT, P. (1990). Transitive inferences by young children about spatial position. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 497–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. PETRY, N., & CASARELLA, T. (1999). Excessive discounting of delayed rewards in substance abusers with gambling problems. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 56, 25–32.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. PIAGET, J., & INHELDER, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  39. RACHLIN, H. (2000). The science of self-control. Cambridge, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  40. REYNOLDS, B., DE WIT, H., & RICHARDS, J. B. (2002). Delay of gratification and delay discounting in rats. Behavioural Processes, 59, 157–168.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. REYNOLDS, B., & KARRAKER, K. (2003). A Big Five model of disposition and situation interaction: why a “helpful” person may not always behave helpfully. New Ideas in Psychology, 21, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. REYNOLDS, B., KARRAKER, K., HORN, K., & RICHARDS, J. B. (2003). Delay and probability discounting as related to different stages of adolescent smoking and non-smoking. Behavioural Processes, 64, 333–344.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. REYNOLDS, B., RICHARDS, J. B., HORN, K., & KARRAKER, K. (2004). Delay discounting and probability discounting as related to cigarette smoking status in adults. Behavioural Processes, 65, 35–42.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. REYNOLDS, B., & SCHIFFBAUER, R. (2004). Measuring state changes in human delay discounting: An experiential discounting task. Behavioural Processes, 67, 343–356.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. REYNOLDS, B., SCHIFFBAUER, R., RICHARDS, J. B., & KARRAKER, K. (2003, May). Development of a task measure of hyperbolic discounting in children: The experiential discounting task-child (Edt-C). Poster presented at the 29th annual Association for Behavior Analysis Convention, San Francisco, Ca.Google Scholar
  46. REYNOLDS, B., SCHIFFBAUER, R., SWENSON, L., & KARRAKER, K. (2001, March). Delay discounting: Testing an adjusting-amount procedure with children. Poster presented at the 47th annual Southeastern Psychological Association Convention, Atlanta, Ga.Google Scholar
  47. RICHARDS, J. B., CHOCK, M. A., & DE WIT, H. (1998). Depletion of serotonin causes impairments in sustained inhibition in rats. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 24(1): 11–83.Google Scholar
  48. RICHARDS, J. B., ZHANG, L., MITCHELL, S., & DEWIT, H. (1999). Delay and probibility discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: Effect of alcohol. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 121–143.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  49. RODRIGUEZ, M. L., MISCHEL, W., & SHODA, Y. (1989). Cognitive person variable in delay of gratification of older children at risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 358–367CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. ROGOFF, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  51. SCARR, S. (1997). The development of individual differences in intelligence and personality. In H. Reese & M. Franzen (Eds.), Biological and neuropsychological mechanisms: Life-span developmental psychology (pp. 1–22). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  52. SCHWEITZER, J., & SULZER-AZAROFF, B. (1995). Self-control in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Effects of added stimulation and time. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 36, 671–686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. SETHI, A., MISCHEL, W., ABER, L., SHODA, Y., & RODRIGUEZ, M. (2000). The role of strategic attention development in development of self-regulation: Predicting preschoolers’ delay of gratification from mother-toddler interactions. Developmental Psychology, 36, 767–777.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. SHODA, Y., MISCHEL, W., & PEAKE, P. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: Indentifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26, 978–986.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. SKINNER, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: D. Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
  56. SMITH, L. (1982). Class inclusion and conclusions about Piaget’s theory. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 267–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. VOGAL-SPROTT, M., EASDON, G., FILLMORE, M., FINN, R., & JUSTUS, A. (2001). Alcohol and behavioral control: Cognitive and neural mechanisms. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 25, 117–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. VUCHINICH, R., & SIMPSON, C. (1998). Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacoloty, 6, 292–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. WADE, T. R., DE WIT, H., & RICHARDS, J. B. (2000). Effects of dopaminergic drugs on delayed reward as a measure of impulsive behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology, 150, 90–101.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.The University of ChicagoUSA
  2. 2.West Virginia UniversityUSA

Personalised recommendations