Acta Theriologica

, Volume 48, Issue 2, pp 167–176 | Cite as

Recognition of interspecific familiar versus unfamiliar odours among bank voles and yellow-necked mice

Article

Abstract

Two experiments (laboratory and field-laboratory) were designed to determine whether individual bank volesClethrionomys glareolus (Schreber, 1780) and yellow--necked miceApodemus flavicollis (Melchior, 1834) could distinguish heterospecific odour cues from familiar and unfamiliar individuals. In the laboratory experiment each male bank vole was familiarized for 24 h with odour (cotton wool impregnated with urine and faeces) of male yellow-necked mice and yellow-necked mice were familiarized with odour of male bank voles. In the field-laboratory experiment the individual bank voles and yellow-necked mice captured at the same point were considered familiar and transfered to the laboratory. In laboratory, these individuals were tested in a box (for 5 h) affording them the choice between the odours of familiar and unfamiliar heterospecific males. Bank voles discriminated between familiar and unfamiliar yellow--necked mouse odours. Male yellow-necked mice seemed to have a similar ability to recognise odours of familiar bank voles. It is proposed that interactions between these two species occur not only on the species level, but also on the level of individual. This phenomenon (probably asymmetric) can play an important role in spatial orientation, and influence direct contacts between individuals of these species.

Key words

Apodemus flavicollis Clethrionomys glareolus recognition of individuals odour communication familiarity 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Andrzejewski R. 2002. The home-range concept in rodents revised. Acta Theriologica 47, Suppl. 1: 81–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andrzejewski R., Babińska-Werka J., Liro A., Owadowska E. and Szacki J. 1997. The attractiveness of conspecific and interspecific odour for bank volesClethrionomys glareolus. Acta Theriologica 42: 231–234.Google Scholar
  3. Andrzejewski R., Babińska-Werka J., Liro A., Owadowska E. and Szacki J. 2000. Homing and space activity in bank voles Clethrionomys glareolus. Acta Theriologica 45: 155–165.Google Scholar
  4. Andrzejewski R. and Olszewski J. 1963. Social behaviour and interspecific relations inApodemus flavicollis (Melchior, 1834) andClethrionomys glareolus (Schreber, 1870). Acta Theriologica 7: 155–168.Google Scholar
  5. Andrzejewski R. and Owadowska E. 1994. Use of odour bait to catch bank voles. Acta Theriologica 39: 221–225.Google Scholar
  6. Babińska-Werka J. 1990. Response of rodents to an increased and quantitatively diverse food base. Acta Theriologica 35: 151–156.Google Scholar
  7. Beauchamp G. K., Yamazaki K., Wysocki C. J., Slotnicki B. M., Thomas L. and Boyse E. A. 1985. Chemosensory recognition of mouse major histocompatibility types by another species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 82: 4186–4188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Benhamou S. 1989. An olfactory orientation model for mammals’ movements in their home ranges. Journal of Theoretical Biology 139: 379–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bergsted B. 1965. Distribution, reproduction, growth and dynamics of the rodents species Clethrionomys glareolus (Schreber),Apodemus flavicollis (Melchior) andApodemus sylvaticus (Linné) in southern Sweden. Oikos 16: 132–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bergstedt B. 1966. Home ranges and movements of the rodent species Clethrionomys glareolus (Schreber),Apodemus flavicollis (Melchior) andApodemus sylvaticus (Linné) in southern Sweden. Oikos 17: 150–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bowers J. M. and Alexander B. K. 1967. Mice: individual recognition by olfaction cues. Science, N. Y. 158: 1208–1210.Google Scholar
  12. Brisbin I. L. and Austad S. N. 1991. Testing the individual odour theory of canine olfaction. Animal Behaviour 42: 63–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bronson F. H. 1974. Pheromonal influences on reproductive activities in rodents. [In: Pheromones. M. C. Birch, ed]. North-Holland Publishing Company, Amsterdam: 344–365.Google Scholar
  14. Chełkowska H., Walkowa W. and Adamczyk K. 1985. Spatial relationships in sympatric populations of the rodents:Clethrionomys glareolus, Microtus agrestis andApodemus agrarius. Acta Theriologica 30: 51–78.Google Scholar
  15. Dickman C. R. and Doncaster C. P. 1989. The ecology of small mammals in urban habitats. II. Demography and dispersal. Journal of Animal Ecology 58: 119–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Drickamer L. C. 1984. Captures of two species ofPeromyscus at live traps baited with male and female odors. Journal of Mammology 65: 699–702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Drickamer L. C., Mikesic D. G. and Shaffer K. S. 1992. Use of odor baits in traps to test reactions to intra- and interspecific chemical cues in house mice living in outdoor enclosures. Journal of Chemical Ecology 18: 2223–2250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fasola M. and Canova L. 2000. Asymmetrical competition between the bank vole and the wood mouse, a removal experiment. Acta Theriologica 45: 353–365.Google Scholar
  19. Ferkin M. H. 1990. Odour selections of island beach voles during their non breeding season. Journal of Mammology 71: 397–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Ferkin M. H. and Johnston R. E. 1995. Meadow voles,Microtus pennsylvanicus, use multiple sources of scent for sexual recognition. Animal Behaviour 49: 37–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Finley R. B. 1959. Observation of nocturnal animals by red light. Journal of Mammalogy 40: 591–594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gilbert A. N., Yamazaki K., Beauchamp G. K. and Thomas L. 1986. Human olfactory discrimination of the mouse (Mus musculus) strains and major histocompatibility types. Journal of Comparative Psychology 100: 262–265.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Greenwood P. J. 1978. Timing of activity of the bank vole (Clethrionomys glareolus) and the wood mouse (Apodemus sylvaticus) in a deciduous woodland. Oikos 31: 123–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Grüm L. and Bujalska G. 2000. Bank voles and yellow-necked mice: what are interrelations between them? [In: Bank vole biology: recent advances in the population biology of a model species. G. Bujalska and L. Hanson, eds]. Polish Journal of Ecology 48, Suppl.: 141–145.Google Scholar
  25. Hoffmeyer I. 1982. Responses of female bank voles (Clethrionomys glareolus) to dominant vs. subordinate conspecific males and the urine odours from dominant and subordinate males. Behavioral and Neural Biology 36: 178–188.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Jamon M. 1994. An analysis of trail-following behaviour in the wood mouse,Apodemus sylvaticus. Animal Behaviour 47: 1127–1134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Jędrzejewski W., Rychlik L. and Jêdrzejewska B. 1993. Responses of bank voles to odours of seven species of predators: experimental data and their relevance to natural predator-vole relationships. Oikos 68: 251–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Johnston R. E. and Bullock T. A. 2001. Individual recognition by use of odours in golden hamsters: the nature of individual representations. Animal Behaviour 61: 545–557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kalinowska A. 1971. Trapping ofApodemus flavicollis andClethrionomys glareolus into a double trap. Acta Theriologica 16: 73–78.Google Scholar
  30. Kalmus H. 1955. The discrimination by the nose of the dog of individual human odours and in particular the odours of twins. British Journal of Animal Behaviour 3: 25–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kałkowski W. 1967. Olfactory bases of social orientation in the white mouse. Folia Biologica 15: 69–86.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Kareem A. M. and Barnard C. J. 1982. The importance of kinship and familiarity in social interactions between mice. Animal Behaviour 30: 594–601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kruczek M. 1994. Reactions of female bank volesClethrionomys glareolus to male chemosignals. Acta Theriologica 39: 249–255.Google Scholar
  34. Lambin X. and Bauchau V. 1989. Contest competition between wood mice and bank voles: is there a winner? Acta Theriologica 34: 385–390.Google Scholar
  35. Lavenex P. and Schenk F. 1998. Olfactory traces and spatial learning in rats. Animal Behaviour 56: 1129–1136.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Marchlewska-Koj A. 2000. Olfactory and ultrasonic communication in bank voles. [In: Bank vole biology: recent advances in the population biology of a model species. G. Bujalska and L. Hanson, eds]. Polish Journal of Ecology 48, Suppl.: 11–20.Google Scholar
  37. Montgomery W. 1979. An examination of interspecific, sexual and individual biases affecting rodent captures in Longworth traps. Acta Theriologica 24: 35–45.Google Scholar
  38. Owadowska E. 1999. The range of olfactory familiarity between individuals in a population of bank voles. Acta Theriologica 44: 133–150.Google Scholar
  39. Radda A. 1969. Studies on the home ranges ofApodemus flavicollis (Melch. 1834). Zoologické Listy 18: 11–22.Google Scholar
  40. Rajska-Jurgiel E. 1976. Interactions between individuals of a population of the bank vole,Clethrionomys glareolus (Schreber, 1780). Ekologia Polska 24: 3–35.Google Scholar
  41. Settle R. H., Sommerville B. A., McComick J. and Broom D. M. 1994. Human scent matching using specially trained dogs. Animal Behaviour 48: 1443–1448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Shapiro L. E., Austin D., Ward S. E. and Dewsbury D. A. 1986. Familiarity and female mate choice in two species of voles (Microtus ochrogaster andMicrotus montanus). Animal Behaviour 34: 90–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Stoddart M. D. 1974. The role of odour in the social biology of small mammals. [In: Pheromones. M. C. Birch, ed]. North-Holland Publishing Company, Amsterdam: 297–315.Google Scholar
  44. Stoddart M. D. and Smith P. A. 1984. Woodmice (Apodemus sylvaticus) can distinguish conspecific from heterospecific odours in the field. Journal of Chemical Ecology 10: 923–928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Stoddart M. D. and Smith P. A. 1986. Recognition of odour-inducted bias in the live-trapping ofApodemus sylvaticus. Oikos 46: 194–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Szacki J. and Liro A. 1991. Movements of small mammals in the heterogenous landscape. Landscape Ecology 5: 219–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Vestal B. M. and Hellack J. J. 1978. Comparison of neighbor recognition in two species of deer mice (Peromyscus). Journal of Mammalogy 59: 339–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Wolton R. J. 1984. Individual recognition by olfaction in the wood mouse,Apodemus sylvaticus. Behaviour 88: 191–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Wolton R. J. 1985. The ranging and nesting behaviour of wood mice,Apodemus sylvaticus (Rodentia, Muridae). Journal of Zoology, London 206: 203–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Wójcik J. M. and Wołk K. 1985. The daily activity rhythm of two competitive rodents:Clethrionomys glareolus andApodemus flavicollis. Acta Theriologica 30: 241–258.Google Scholar
  51. Zejda J. 2002. Yellow-necked mouse attacked a dead bank vole. Acta Theriologica 47: 221–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Zejda J. and Pelikan J. 1969. Movements and home ranges of some rodents in lowland forests. Zoologické Listy 18: 143–162.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Mammal Research Institute, Bialowieza, Poland 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Catholic University of LublinLublinPoland

Personalised recommendations