The impact of early metalinguistic competencies and memory capacity on reading and spelling in elementary school: Results of the Munich Longitudinal Study on the Genesis of Individual Competencies (LOGIC)

  • Wolfgang Schneider
  • Jan Carol Näslund
Article

Abstract

This paper reports on a longitudinal study dealing with the development of literacy in young children. A total of 163 children were first tested during their last year in kindergarten using a variety of tasks that tapped phonological processing, memory capacity, early literacy, and intelligence. Children’s word decoding, reading comprehension, and spelling skills were assessed in elementary school several years later. As a main result, all of the predictor domains had a significant impact on the acquisition of literacy in elementary school, although the contribution of each domain differed as a function of the criterion measure. An attempt to identify children-at-risk using a kindergarten screening test provided encouraging results. Nonetheless, it was shown that whereas group predictions of reading and spelling performance can be quite accurate, the individual prognosis of school problems is far from perfect.

Key words

Acquisition of literacy Children-at-Risk Memory capacity Phonological awareness 

References

  1. Blachman, B. A. (1984). Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills to kindergarten and first-grade reading achievement.Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 610–622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1985).Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
  3. Brügelmann, H. (1986).Lese- und Schreibaufgaben für Schulanfänger (Assessment of reading and writing in beginning school children). Technical Report, University of Bremen.Google Scholar
  4. Burgemeister, B., Blum, L., & Lorge, J. (1972).Columbia Mental Maturity Scale. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  5. Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid ‘automatized’ naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities.Neuropsychologia, 14, 471–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Case, R., Kurland, D. M., & Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 386–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Daneman, M., & Blennerhassett, A. (1984). How to assess the listening comprehension skills of prereaders.Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 1372–1381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Eggert, D. (1978).Hannover-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für das Vorschulalter (Hannover-Wechsler intelligence test for preschoolers and kindergarteners). Bern, Switzerland: Huber-Verlag.Google Scholar
  9. Ehri, L. C. (1989). the development of spelling knowledge and its role in reading acquisition and reading disability.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 356–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Ellis, N., & Large, B. (1987). The development of reading: As you seek so shall you find.British Journal of Psychology, 78, 1–28.Google Scholar
  11. Horn, W. F., & Packard, T. (1985). Early identification of learning problems: A meta-analysis.Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 597–607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Jansen, H., Knorn, P., Mannhaupt, G., Marx, H., Beck, M., & Skowronek, H. (1986).Bielefelder Screening zur Vorhersage von Lese- und Rechtschreibschwierigkeiten (The Bielefeld Screening test for the prediction of reading and spelling difficulties). Technical Report, University of Bielefeld.Google Scholar
  13. Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1984).LISREL IV — Analysis of linear structural relationships by m maximum likelihood instrumental variables, and least squares methods (User’s Guide). Mooresville, IN: Scientific Software.Google Scholar
  14. Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades.Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Petersen, O. P. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children.Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 261–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lundberg, I., & Hoien, T. (1991). Initial enabling knowledge and skills in reading acquisition: Print awareness and phonological segmentation. In D. J. Sawyer & B. J. Fox (Eds.),Phonological awareness in reading, (pp. 73–96). New York: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
  17. Marx, H. (1992).Vorhersage von Lese-rechtschreibschwierigkeiten in Theorie und Anwendung (The prediction of reading and spelling problems in theory and practice) Unpublished habilitation, University of Bielefeld.Google Scholar
  18. Morais, J. (1991). Phonological awareness: A bridge between language and literacy. In D. J. Sawyer & B. J. Fox (Eds.),Phonological awareness in reading, (pp. 31–72). New York: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
  19. Morais, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J., & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously?Cognition, 7, 323–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Näslund, J. C. (1990). The interrelationships among preschool predictors of reading acquisition for German children.Reading and Writing, 2, 327–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Näslund, J. C., & Schneider, W. (1991). Longitudinal effects of verbal ability, memory capacity, and phonological awareness on reading performance.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 6, 375–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Näslund, J. C., & Schneider, W. (1993).How do letter knowledge and phonological skills in kindergarten affect later reading and spelling performance? Paper submitted for publication. University of New Mexico.Google Scholar
  23. Perfetti, C. A., Beck, I., Bell, L. C., & Hughes, C. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 255–281.Google Scholar
  24. Perfetti, C. A., & Lesgold, A. M. (1978). Coding and comprehension in skilled reading and instruction. In L. B. Resnick & P. A. Weaver (Eds.),Theory and practice of early reading, Vol. 1, (pp. 57–84). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  25. Read, C., Yun-Fei, Z., Hong-Yin, N., & Bao-Quing, D. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic writing.Cognition, 24, 31–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Rott, C., & Zielinski, W. (1986). Entwicklung der Lesefertigkeit in der Grundschule (The development of reading skills in elementary school).Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 18, 165–175.Google Scholar
  27. Schneider, W. (1992).Möglichkeiten und Probleme bei der frühen Vorhersage von Lese-Rechtschreibleistungen (Perspectives and problems related to the early prediction of reading and spelling). Paper presented at the biennial meetings of the German Psychology Association, Trier.Google Scholar
  28. Schneider, W., & Näslund, J. C. (1992). Cognitive prerequisites of reading and spelling: A longitudinal approach. In A. Demetriou, M. Shayer, & A. Efklides (Eds.),Neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development: Implications and applications for education, (pp. 256–274). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  29. Skowronek, H., & Marx, H. (1989). The Bielefeld longitudinal study on early identification of risks in learning to write and read: Theoretical background and first results. In M. Brambring, F. Lösel, & H. Skowronek (Eds.),Children at risk: Assessment, longitudinal research, and intervention, (pp. 268–294). New York: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
  30. Stanovich, K., Cunningham, A. E., & Feeman, D. J. (1984). Intelligence, cognitive skills, and early reading progress.Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 278–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Tramontana, M. G., Hooper, S. R., & Selzer,S. C. (1988). Research on the preschool prediction of later academic achievement: A review.Developmental Review, 8, 89–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Tunmer, W. E., & Nesdale, A. R. (1985). Phonemic segmentation skill and beginning reading.Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 417–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Wagner, R., & Torgesen, J. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills.Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Weinert, F. E., & Schneider, W. (Eds.) (1987).The Munich Longitudinal Study on the Genesis of Individual Competencies (LOGIC), Report No. 2: Documentation of assessment procedures used in waves one to three (Technical Report). Munich: Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research.Google Scholar
  35. Weinert, F. E., & Schneider, W. (Eds.) (1992).The Munich Longitudinal Study on the Genesis of Individual Competencies (LOGIC), Report No. 8: Assessment procedures and results of wave five (Technical Report). Munich: Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Instituto Superior de Psicologia Aplicada, Lisbon, Portugal/ Springer Netherlands 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Wolfgang Schneider
    • 1
  • Jan Carol Näslund
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of WürzburgWürzburgGermany
  2. 2.College of EducationUniversity of New MexikoAlbuquerqueUSA

Personalised recommendations