The heart rate component of the social reflex in dogs: The conditional effects of petting and person

  • James J. Lynch
  • W. Horsley Gantt
Article

Abstract

Five dogs were given a 480-cps tone followed by shock for more than 200 trials. A 200-cps tone was then introduced followed by a person petting the dog at the end of the conditional signal (CS) for 50 trials. Finally, the 480-cps tone-shock (T-S) sequence was reintroduced for five trials, after which the person entered the experimental room and stood beside the dog during one additional T-S sequence.

It was observed that both the heart rate (HR) increase to shock, and the HR decrease to petting could be conditioned rapidly (1–5 trials). These findings are discussed in terms of the theory of stimulus substitution in classical HR conditioning.

After the person had been made part of a CS for petting, and the T-S sequence reintroduced, the HR increase during the CS was reduced. The HR response to the shock, however, was greater than the response to this US when the dog was alone. A control group, given the same T-S sequence with a person present who had not been a CS for petting, did not show any significant HR changes from the usual response given to the T-S sequence.

It is emphasized that these findings, in conjunction with earlier reports, indicate that the cardiovascular system may be a valuable index in studying the psychophysiology of socialization processes.

Keywords

Conditional Signal Stimulus Substitution Person Present Heart Rate Difference Conditional Bradycardia 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anderson, S., and Gantt, W. H.: The effect of person on cardiac and motor responsivity to shock in dogs.Cond. Reflex,1: 181–189, 1966.Google Scholar
  2. Fisher, A. E.: The effects of differential early treatment on the social and exploratory behavior of puppies. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University, 1955.Google Scholar
  3. Fuller, J. L.: Effects of experimental deprivation upon behavior in animals.World Cong. Psychiat.,3: 223–227, 1963.Google Scholar
  4. Gantt, W. H., Newton, J. E. O., Royer, F. L., and Stephens, J. H.: Effect of person.Cond. Reflex,1: 18–35, 1966.Google Scholar
  5. Hebb, D. O.: The mammal and his environment.Am. J. Psychiat.,111: 826, 1955.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Lynch, J. J.: The cardiac orienting response and its relationship to the cardiac conditional response in dogs.Cond. Reflex,2: 138–152, 1967.Google Scholar
  7. Lynch, J. J.: Heart rate variability of dogs in classical conditioning.Psychol. Record, January, 1968.Google Scholar
  8. Lynch, J. J., and McCarthy, J. F.: The effect of petting on a classically conditioned emotional response.Behav. Res. Ther.,5: 55–62, 1967.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Lynch, J. J., and McCarthy, J. F.: Social responding in dogs: Heart rate changes to a person.Psychophysiology. In press.Google Scholar
  10. McDonald, D. G., Stern, J. A., and Hahn, W. W.: Classical heart rate conditioning in the rat.J. Psychosom. Res.,7: 97–106, 1963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. McIntire, R. W., and Colley, T. A.: Social reinforcement in the dog.Psychol. Rep.,20: 843–846, 1967.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Melzack, R., and Scott, T. H.: The effects of early experience on the response to pain.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.,50: 155–161, 1957.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Murphee, O. D., Peters, J. E., and Dykman, R. A.: Effect of person on nervous, stable and crossbred pointer dogs.Conditional Reflex,2: 273–276, 1967.Google Scholar
  14. Notterman, J. M., Schoenfeld, W. H., and Bersh, P. J.: Conditioned heart rate response in human being during experimental anxiety.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.,45: 1–8, 1952.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Pavlov, I. P.:Lectures on Conditioned Reflexes. W. H. Gantt, Trans. New York, International Publishers, 1928, p. 368.Google Scholar
  16. Pavlov, I. P.:Conditioned Reflexes. G. Anrep, Tr., Oxford Press, 1927.Google Scholar
  17. Royer, F. L., and Gantt, W. H.: The effect of different persons on the heart rate of dogs. Eastern Psychol. Assoc., Philadelphia, Pa., March, 1961.Google Scholar
  18. Scott, J. P.: Social behavior organization and leadership in a small flock of domestic sheep.Comp. Psych. Monograph, No. 96,18: 1–29, 1945.Google Scholar
  19. Scott, J. P.: The social behavior of dogs and wolves: An illustration of sociobiological systematics.Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.,51: 1009–1021, 1950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Scott, J. P.: The analysis of social organization in animals.Ecology,37: 213–221, 1956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Stanley, W. C.: The passive person as a reinforcer in isolated Beagle puppies.Psychon. Sci.,2: 21–22, 1965.Google Scholar
  22. Stanley, W. C., Morris, D. D., and Trattner, A.: Conditioning with a passive person reinforcer and extinction in Shetland sheep dog puppies.Psychon. Sci.,2: 19–20, 1966.Google Scholar
  23. Thompson, W. R., and Heron, W.: The effects of early restriction on activity in dogs.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.,47: 77–82, 1954.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Zeaman, D., and Smith, R. W.: Review of some recent findings in human cardiac conditioning.In Prokasy, W. F.:Classical Conditioning: A Symposium. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1965, p. 378–418.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer 1968

Authors and Affiliations

  • James J. Lynch
    • 1
  • W. Horsley Gantt
    • 1
  1. 1.The Pavlovian LaboratoryThe Johns Hopkins University School of MedicineBaltimore

Personalised recommendations