Annals of Behavioral Medicine

, Volume 21, Issue 3, pp 216–222 | Cite as

Negative emotions and acute physiological responses to stress

  • Pamela J. Feldman
  • Sheldon Cohen
  • Stephen J. Lepore
  • Karen A. Matthews
  • Thomas W. Kamarck
  • Anna L. Marsland
Article

Abstract

One pathway through which stressors are thought to influence physiology is through their effects on emotion. We used meta-analytic statisitical techniques with data from nine studies to test the effects of acute laboratory stressors (speech, star mirror-image tracing, handgrip) on emotional (undifferentiated negative emotion, anger, anxiety) and cardiovascular (CV) response. In all of the studies, participants responded to stressors with both increased CV response and increased negative emotion. Increases in negative emotion were associated with increases in CV response across tasks, however, these associations were small. The range of variance accounted for was between 2% and 12%. Thus, the contribution of negative emotion, as assessed in these studies, to physiological responses to acute laboratory stressors was limited. Although these results raise questions about the role of emotion in mediating stress-elicited physiological responses, the nature of the acute laboratory stress paradigm may contribute to the lack of a strong association.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. (1).
    Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU (eds):Measuring Stress: A Guide for Health and Social Scientists. New York: Oxford University Press, 1995.Google Scholar
  2. (2).
    Lazarus R, Folkman S:Stress, Appraisal and Coping. New York: Springer, 1984.Google Scholar
  3. (3).
    Mason JW: A historical view of the stress field: Part II.Journal of Human Stress. 1975,1:22–36.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. (4).
    Schneiderman N, McCabe PM: Psychophysiologic strategies in laboratory research. In Schneiderman N, Weiss SM, Kaufmann PG (eds),Handbook of Research Methods in Cardiovascular Behavioral Medicine. New York: Plenum, 1989, 349–364.Google Scholar
  5. (5).
    Krantz DS, Manuck SB: Acute physiologic reactivity and risk of cardiovascular disease: A review and methodologic critique.Psychological Bulletin. 1984,96:435–464.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. (6).
    Obrist PA:Cardiovascular Psychophysiology: A Perspective. New York: Plenum, 1981.Google Scholar
  7. (7).
    Lacey JI, Lacey BC: Some autonomic-central nervous system interrelationships. In Black H (ed),Physiological Correlates of Emotion. New York: Academic Press, 1970, 205–227.Google Scholar
  8. (8).
    Rosenthal R:Meta-Analytic Procedures for Social Research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage, 1984.Google Scholar
  9. (9).
    Allen MT, Stoney CM, Owens JF, Matthews KA: Hemodynamic adjustments to laboratory stress: The influence of gender and personality.Psychosomatic Medicine. 1993,55:505–517.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. (10).
    Cohen S, Hamrick N, Rodriguez M, et al: The stability of and interrelations between cardiovascular, immune, endocrine, and psychological reactivity. Manuscript submitted for publication, 1999.Google Scholar
  11. (11).
    Kamarck TW, Peterman AH, Raynor DA: The effects of the social environment on stress-related cardiovascular activation: Current findings, prospects, and implications.Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 1998,20(4):247–256.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. (12).
    Lepore S: Cynicism, social support, and cardiovascular reactivity.Health Psychology. 1995,14:210–216.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. (13).
    Marsland AL, Manuck SB, Fazzari TV, Stewart CJ, Rabin BS: Stability of individual differences in cellular immune responses to acute psychological stress.Psychosomatic Medicine. 1995,57: 295–298.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. (14).
    Matthews KA, Berga SL, Owens JF, Flory JD: Effects of short-term suppression of ovarian hormones on cardiovascular and neuroendocrine reactivity to stress in women.Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1998,23:307–322.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. (15).
    Owens JF, Stoney CM, Matthews KA: Menopausal status influences ambulatory blood pressure levels and blood pressure changes during mental stress.Circulation. 1993,88:2794–2802.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. (16).
    Stoney CM, Owens JF, Guzick DS, Matthews KA: A natural experiment on the effects of ovarian hormones on cardiovascular risk factors and stress reactivity: Bilateral salpingo-oophorectomy versus hysterectomy only.Health Psychology. 1997,16:349–358.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. (17).
    McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF:Manual for the Profile of Mood States. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service, 1971.Google Scholar
  18. (18).
    King MG, Burrows GD, Stanley GV: Measurement of stress and arousal: Validation of the stress/arousal adjective checklist.British Journal of Psychology. 1983,74:473–479.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. (19).
    Cohen J, Cohen P:Applied Multiple Regression/Correlational Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1983.Google Scholar
  20. (20).
    Cohen J: A power primer.Psychological Bulletin. 1992,112: 155–159.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. (21).
    Schwartz GE, Weinberger DA, Singer JA: Cardiovascular differentiation of sadness, anger, and fear, following imagery and exercise.Psychosomatic Medicine. 1981,43:343–364.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. (22).
    Sinha R, Lovallo WR, Parsons OA: Cardiovascular differentiation of emotions.Psychosomatic Medicine. 1992,54:422–435.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. (23).
    Ekman P: Expression and the nature of emotion. In Scherer KS, Ekman P (eds),Approaches to Emotion. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1984, 319–343.Google Scholar
  24. (24).
    LeDoux JE: Emotion: Clues from the brain.Annual Review of Psychology. 1995,46:209–235.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. (25).
    Zajonc R: On the primacy of emotion.American Psychologist. 1984,39:117–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. (26).
    Lazarus R: On the primacy of cognition.American Psychologist. 1984,39:124–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. (27).
    Schachter S, Singer JE: Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state.Psychological Review. 1962,69:379–399.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. (28).
    Stone AA: Measurement of affective response. In Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU (eds),Measuring Stress: A Guide for Health and Social Scientists. New York: Oxford University Press, 1995, 148–174.Google Scholar
  29. (29).
    Cacioppo JT, Petty RE, Losch ME, Kim HS: Electromyographic activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate the valence and intensity of affective reactions.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986,50:260–268.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. (30).
    Ekman P, Friesen WV: Felt, false, and miserable smiles.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 1982,6:238–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. (31).
    Gottman JM:What Predicts Divorce: The Relationship Between Marital Processes and Marital Outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1994.Google Scholar
  32. (32).
    Lorr M: Models and methods for the measurement of mood. In Plutchik R, Kellerman H (eds),Emotion: Theory, Research and Experience (Vol. 4). New York: Academic Press, 1989, 37–53.Google Scholar
  33. (33).
    Light KC, Obrist PA: Cardiovascular response to stress: Effects of opportunity to avoid shock experience and performance feedback.Psychophysiology. 1980,17:243–252.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. (34).
    Sherwood A, Dolan CA, Light KC: Hemodynamics of blood pressure responses during active and passive coping.Psychophysiology. 1990,27:656–668.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. (35).
    Houston BK: Personality dimensions in reactivity and cardiovascular disease. In Schneiderman N, Weiss SM, Kaufmann PG (eds),Handbook of Research Methods in Cardiovascular Behavioral Medicine. New York: Plenum, 1989, 495–510.Google Scholar
  36. (36).
    Cacioppo JT, Uchino BN, Crites SL, et al: Relationship between facial expressiveness and sympathetic activation in emotion: A critical review, with emphasis on modeling underlying mechanisms and individual differences.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992,62:110–128.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. (37).
    Matthews K, Owens JF, Allen MT, Stoney CM: Do cardiovascular responses to laboratory stress relate to ambulatory blood pressure levels?: Yes, in some people, some of the time.Psychosomatic Medicine. 1992,54:686–697.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. (38).
    Christensen AJ, Smith TW: Cynical hostility and cardiovascular reactivity during self-disclosure.Psychosomatic Medicine. 1993,55:193–202.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. (39).
    Miller GE, Dopp JM, Myers HF, Stevens SY, Fahey JL: Psychosocial predictors of natural killer cell mobilization during marital conflict.Health Psychology. 1999,18(3):262–271.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. (40).
    Suarez EC, Harlan E, Peoples MC, Williams RB: Cardiovascular and emotional responses in women: The role of hostility and harassment.Health Psychology. 1993,12:459–468.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. (41).
    Suarez EC, Williams RB: Situational determinants of cardiovascular and emotional responses in high and low hostile men.Psychosomatic Medicine. 1989,52:558–570.Google Scholar
  42. (42).
    Williams RB: Neurobiology, cellular and molecular biology, and psychosomatic medicine.Psychosomatic Medicine. 1994,56: 308–315.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. (43).
    Ney T, Gale A: A critique of laboratory studies of emotion with particular reference to psychophysiological aspects. In Wagner HL (ed),Social Psychophysiology and Emotion: Theory and Clinical Applications. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, 1988, 65–83.Google Scholar
  44. (44).
    Levenson RW, Carstensen LL, Gottman JM: The influence of age and gender on emotion, physiology, and their interrelations: A study of long-term marriages.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994,67:56–68.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. (45).
    Kamarck TW, Shiffman SM, Smithline L, et al: Effects of task strain, social conflict, and emotional activation on ambulatory cardiovascular activity: Daily life consequences of recurring stress in a multiethnic adult sample.Health Psychology. 1998,17:17–29.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. (46).
    Southard DR, Coates TJ, Kolodner K, et al: Relationship between mood and blood pressure in the natural environment: An adolescent population.Health Psychology. 1986,5:469–480.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. (47).
    Gellman M, Spitzer S, Ironson G, et al: Posture, place, and mood effects on ambulatory blood pressure.Psychophysiology. 1990,27:544–551.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. (48).
    James GD, Yee LS, Harshfield GA, Pickering TG: Sex differences in factors affecting the daily variation of blood pressure.Social Science and Medicine. 1988,26:1019–1023.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. (49).
    James GD, Yee LS, Harshfield GA, Blank SG, Pickering TG: The influence of happiness, anger, and anxiety on the blood pressure of borderline hypertensives.Psychosomatic Medicine. 1986,48: 502–508.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. (50).
    Stone AA, Shiffman S: Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in behavioral medicine.Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 1994,16: 199–202.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The Society of Behavioral Medicine 1999

Authors and Affiliations

  • Pamela J. Feldman
    • 1
  • Sheldon Cohen
    • 1
  • Stephen J. Lepore
    • 2
  • Karen A. Matthews
    • 3
  • Thomas W. Kamarck
    • 3
  • Anna L. Marsland
    • 4
  1. 1.Carnegie Mellon UniversityPittsburghUSA
  2. 2.Brooklyn CollegeCity University of New YorkBrookhynUSA
  3. 3.University of PittsburghPittsburghUSA
  4. 4.University of Pittsburgh Medical CenterPittsburghUSA

Personalised recommendations