International Journal of Primatology

, Volume 16, Issue 1, pp 1–16 | Cite as

Innovation in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)

  • Christophe Boesch
Important Announcement

Abstract

Innovations of behavior have major implications for the concept of culture in animals. Innovation has rarefy been documented in wild animal populations. The chimpanzees of the Taï National Park, Côte d’Ivoire, spontaneously included new patterns of behavior in their repertoire during our study. Some innovations were incorporated into a minority of the group members’ repertoire, whereas others, such as building day nests in trees and on the ground more frequently, became general behaviors. Similarly, new contexts for use of leaf- clipping behavior and novel ways of feeding on some leaves appeared and rapidly became part of the behavior of most group members. The environmental parameters were too stable to explain these new forms of behavior, as most of them took place within 1 month during the same dry season. In a similar way, leaf- grooming acquired a new function in Gombe chimpanzees. A process similar to social conventions could explain the emergence of these new functions for an existing behavior in a way rather similar to human “fashion. ” Three major characteristics of human culture — the absence of individual variations in the performance of the behavior, the general use of the behavior by group members, and the ability to modify the function of a behavior — characterize leaf-clipping in Taï chimpanzees and leaf- grooming in Gombe chimpanzees.

Key words

chimpanzee culture innovation tool-use cross-population comparisons 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Beck, B. (1980).Animal Tool Behavior, Garland Press, New York.Google Scholar
  2. Boesch, C. (1991). Teaching in wild chimpanzees.Anim. Behav. 41(3): 530–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Boesch, C. (1991). The effect of leopard predation on grouping patterns in forest chimpanzees.Behavior 117(3–4): 220–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Boesch, C. (1993). Toward a new image of culture in chimpanzees.Behav. Brain Sci. 16(3): 514–515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boesch, C., and Boesch, H. (1983). Optimization of nut-cracking with natural hammers by wild chimpanzees.Behavior 83: 265–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boesch, C., and Boesch, H. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park.Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 78: 547–573.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Boesch, C., and Boesch, H. (1990). Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees.Folia Primatol. 54: 86–99.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Boesch, C., Marchesi, P., Marchesi, N., Fruth, B. and Joulian, F. (1994). Is nut cracking in wild chimpanzees a cultural behavior?J. Hum. EvoL 26: 325–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cheney, D., and Seyfarth, R. (1990).How Monkeys See the World- Inside the Mind of Another Species, Chicago University Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  10. Fouts, R. S., Hirsch, A. D., and Fouts, D. H. (1982). Cultural transmission of human language in a chimpanzee mother-infant relationship. In Fitzgerald, H. E., Mullins, J. A., and Gage P. (eds.),Psychobiological Perspectives, Plenum, New York, pp. 159–193.Google Scholar
  11. Galef, B. (1990). Tradition in animals: Field observations and laboratory analyses. In Bekoff, M., and Jamieson, D. (eds.),Interpretation and Explanation in the Study of Animal Behavior, Westview Press, Boulder, pp. 74–95.Google Scholar
  12. Goodall, J. (1973). Cultural elements in a chimpanzee community. In Menzel, E.W. (ed.),Precultural Primate Behavior, Fourth IPC Symposia Proceedings, Karger, Basel, Vol. 1, pp. 195–249.Google Scholar
  13. Goodall, J. (1986).The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior, Belknap Press of Havard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  14. Kawai, M. (1965). Newly acquired precultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima islet.Primates 6: 1–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kummer, H. (1971).Primate Societies: Group Techniques of Ecological Adaptation, Aldine, Chicago.Google Scholar
  16. Kummer, H., and Goodall, J. (1985). Conditions of innovative behavior in primates.Phil Trans. R. Soc. Lond B 308: 203–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. McGrew, W. (1992).Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. McGrew, W. (1993). The intelligent use of tools: Twenty propositions. In Gibson, K., and Ingold, T. (eds.),Tools, Language and Intelligence in Human Evolution, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, pp. 151–170.Google Scholar
  19. Nishida T. (1987). Local traditions and cultural transmission. In Smuts, S. S., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., and Strusaker, T. T. (eds.),Primate Societies, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 462–474.Google Scholar
  20. Sugiyama, T. (1981). Observations on the population dynamics and behavior of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea, in 1979–1980.Primates 26: 361–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Thorpe, W. (1956).Learning and Instinct in Animals, Methuen, London.Google Scholar
  22. Tomasello, M. (1990). Cultural transmission in tool use and communicatory signalling of chimpanzees? In Parker, S., and Gibson, K. (eds.),Comparative Developmental Psychology of Language and Intelligence in Primates, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 274–311.Google Scholar
  23. Tomasello, M., Davis-Dasilva, M., Camak, L., and Bard, K. (1987). Observational learning of tool-use by young chimpanzees.J. Hum. Evol. 2: 175–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Tomasello, M. Kruger, A., and Ratner, H. (1993). Cultural learning.Behav. Brain Sci. 16(3): 480–510.Google Scholar
  25. Tomasello, M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., and Kruger, A. (1994). Imitation of object related actions by chimpanzees and human infant.Child Dev. (in press).Google Scholar
  26. Visalberghi, E., and Fragaszy, D. (1990). Do monkeys ape? In Parker, S., and Gibson, K. (eds.),Comparative Developmental Psychology of Language and Intelligence in Primates, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, pp. 247–273.Google Scholar
  27. Whiten, A., and Ham, R. (1992). On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: reappraisal of a century of research. In Slater, P., Rosenblatt, J., and Beer, C. (eds.),Advances in the Study of Behavior, Academic Press, New York, pp. 239–283.Google Scholar
  28. Wrangham, R., and Nishida, T. (1983). Aspilia spp. leaves: A puzzle in the feeding behavior of wild chimpanzees.Primates 24: 276–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Wynn, T. (1993). Layers of thinking in tool behavior. In Ingold, T., and Gibson, K. (eds.),Tools, Language and Intelligence: Evolutionary Implications, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, pp. 389–406.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1995

Authors and Affiliations

  • Christophe Boesch
    • 1
  1. 1.Institute of ZoologyUniversity of BaselBaselSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations