Human Nature

, Volume 5, Issue 3, pp 279–291

A sensitive period for learning about food

  • Elizabeth Cashdan
Article

Abstract

It is proposed here that there is a sensitive period in the first two to three years of life during which humans acquire a basic knowledge of what foods are safe to eat. In support of this, it is shown that willingness to eat a wide variety of foods is greatest between the ages of one and two years, and then declines to low levels by age four. These data also show that children who are introduced to solids unusually late have a narrower diet breadth throughout childhood, perhaps because the duration of the sensitive period has been shortened. By reducing the costs associated with learning, a sensitive period for food learning should be adaptive for any omnivore (including early humans) that remains in the same environment throughout its life.

Key words

Sensitive periods Critical periods Sensitive period learning Food learning Food preferences Weaning 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Birch, L. L. 1979 Dimensions of Preschool Children’s Food Preferences.Journal of Nutrition Education 11:77–80.Google Scholar
  2. 1980 Effects of Peer Models’ Food Choices and Eating Behaviors on Preschoolers’ Food Preferences.Child Development 51:489–496.Google Scholar
  3. 1987 The Acquisition of Food Acceptance Patterns in Children. InEating Habits: Food, Physiology and Learned Behavior, R. Boakes, D. Popplewell, and M. Burton, eds. Pp. 107–130. New York: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
  4. Birch, L. L., and D. W. Marlin 1982 I Don’t Like It; I Never Tried It: Effects of Exposure on 2-Year-Old Children’s Food Preferences.Appetite 3:353–360.Google Scholar
  5. Capretta, P. J., and L. Rawls 1974 Establishment of a Flavor Preference in Rats: Importance of Nursing and Weaning Experience.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 86:670–673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Crawley, M. J. 1983Herbivory: The Dynamics of Animal-Plant Interactions. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  7. Duncker, K. 1938 Experimental Modification of Children’s Food Preferences through Social Suggestion.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 3:489–508.Google Scholar
  8. Fallon, A. E., P. Rozin, and P. Pliner 1984 The Child’s Conception of Food: The Development of Food Rejections with Special Reference to Disgust and Contamination Sensitivity.Child Development 55:566–575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Farb, P., and G. Armelagos 1980Consuming Passions: The Anthropology of Eating. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  10. Fomon, S. J. 1974Infant Nutrition. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.Google Scholar
  11. Galef, B. J. 1991 Innovation in the Study of Social Learning in Animals: Developmental and Biological Perspectives. InDevelopmental Psychobiology: New Methods and Changing Concepts, H. Shair, G. Barr, and M. Hofer, eds. Pp. 114–125. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Galef, B. J., and M. Clark 1971 Social Factors in the Poison Avoidance and Feeding Behavior of Wild and Domesticated Rat Pups.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 5:341–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Galef, B. J., and D. F. Sherry 1973 Mother’s Milk: A Medium for the Transmission of Cues Reflecting the Flavor of Mother’s Diet.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 83:374–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Garcia, J., and R. A. Koelling 1966 Relation of Cue to Consequence in Avoidance Learning.Psychonomic Science 4:123–124.Google Scholar
  15. Harper, L. V., and K. M. Sanders 1975 The Effect of Adults’ Eating on Young Children’s Acceptance of Unfamiliar Foods.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 20:206–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kram, K. M., and G. M. Owen 1972 Nutritional Studies on United States Preschool Children: Dietary Intakes and Practices of Food Procurement, Preparation and Consumption. InPractices of Low-Income Families in Feeding Infants and Small Children, with Particular Attention to Cultural Subgroups, S. Fomon and T. Anderson, eds. HEW Publication No. 72-5605. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.Google Scholar
  17. Lebenthal, E. 1982 Solid Food (Beikost) Consumption and Digestibility in Infancy. InPediatric Nutrition, F. Lifshitz, ed. Pp. 63–69. New York: Marcel Dekker.Google Scholar
  18. Logue, A. W., M. C. Logue, R. Uzzo, M. McCarty, and M. Smith 1988 Food Preferences in Families.Appetite 10:169–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. McKenzie, J. C. 1967 Social and Economic Implications of Minority Food Habits.Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 26:197–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Pelchat, M. L., and P. Pliner 1986 Antecedents and Correlates of Feeding Problems in Young Children.Journal of Nutrition Education 18:23–29.Google Scholar
  21. Peryam, D. R. 1963 The Acceptance of Novel Foods.Food Technology 17:33–39.Google Scholar
  22. Pliner, P. 1982 The Effects of Mere Exposure on Liking for Edible Substances.Appetite 3:283–290.Google Scholar
  23. Pulliam, H. R., and C. Dunford 1979Programmed to Learn: An Essay on the Evolution of Culture. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Rabinowitch, V. 1969 Role of Experience in the Development and Retention of Seed Preferences in Zebra Finches.Behaviour 33:222–236.Google Scholar
  25. Raphael, D. 1979Breastfeeding and Food Policy in a Hungry World. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  26. Raphael, D., and F. Davis 1985Only Mothers Know: Patterns of Infant Feeding in Traditional Cultures. Westport, Connecticut: Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
  27. Rosenthal, G. A., and D. H. Janzen, eds. 1979Herbivores: Their Interaction with Secondary Plant Metabolites. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  28. Rozin, P. 1976 Psychobiological and Cultural Determinants of Food Choice. InAppetite and Food Intake, T. Silverstone, ed. Pp. 285–312. Berlin: Dahlem Konferenzen.Google Scholar
  29. Rozin, P., A. Fallon, and M. Augustoni-Ziskind 1986 The Child’s Conception of Food: The Development of Categories of Acceptable and Rejected Substances.Journal of Nutrition Education 18:75–81.Google Scholar
  30. Rozin, P., L. Hammer, H. Oster, T. Horowitz, and V. Marmora 1986 The Child’s Conception of Food: Differentiation of Categories of Rejected Substances in the 16 Months to 5 Year Age Range.Appetite 7:141–151.Google Scholar
  31. Slater, P. J. B. 1983 The Development of Individual Behavior. InGenes, Development and Learning, T. R. Halliday and P. Slater, eds. Pp. 82–113. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  32. Stephens, D. W. 1990 Risk and Incomplete Information in Behavioral Ecology. InRisk and Uncertainty in Tribal and Peasant Economies, E. Cashdan, ed. Pp. 19–46. Boulder: Westview Press.Google Scholar
  33. 1991 Change, Regularity and Value in the Evolution of Animal Learning.Journal of Behavioural Ecology 2:77–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Symons, D. 1979The Evolution of Human Sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  35. Wilson, E. O. 1975Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Wyrwicka, W. 1981The Development of Food Preferences: Parental Influences and the Primacy Effect. Springfield, Illinois: Charles Thomas.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Walter de Gruyter, Inc 1994

Authors and Affiliations

  • Elizabeth Cashdan
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of AnthropologyUniversity of UtahSalt Lake City

Personalised recommendations