Consciousness: A neurobiological approach

  • Blair H. Turner
  • Margaret E. Knapp
Papers

Abstract

Consciousness and the disorders of consciousness can be more fully understood once they are integrated with the neurobiology of mental events. After a review of animal and human research, we found several anatomical structures in the central nervous system are required for consciousness. Identification of the critical structures, however, depends on what is meant by consciousness. In the general sense of mental responsivity, the reticular activating system must be intact. Consciousness has also been defined as the awareness of the sights, sounds, and feelings of everyday experience. In this sense, the system of sensory inputs and outputs of the anterior temporal cortex, amygdala, and the hippocampus must be functional. There is no neural evidence for “higher” consciousness.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Angrist, B., Lee, H., and Gershon, S. (1974). The antagonism of amphetamine-induced symptomatology by a neuroleptic.American Journal of Psychiatry, 131: 817–819.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Baizer, J., Ungerleider, L., and Desimone, R. (1991). Organization of visual inputs to the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in macaques.Journal of Neuroscience, 11: 168–190.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Gloor, P. (1990). Experiential phenomena of temporal lobe epilepsy.Brain, 113: 1673–1694.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Halgren, E. (1992). Emotional neurophysiology of the amygdala within the context of human cognition. In J.P. Aggleton (Ed.),The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction. New York: Wiley-Liss.Google Scholar
  5. Halgren, E. (1982). Mental phenomena induced by stimulation in the limbic system.Human Neurobiology, 1: 251–260.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Halgren, E., Walter, R., Cherlow, D., and Crandall, P. (1978). Mental phenomena evoked by electrical stimulation of the human hippocampal formation and amygdala.Brain, 101: 83–107.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Kinney, H., Korein, J., Panigrahy, A., Dikkes, P., and Goode, R. (1994). Neuropathological findings in the brain of Karen Ann Quinlan.The New England Journal of Medicine, 330: 1469–1475.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Levine, D.N., Warach, J., and Farah, M. (1985). Two visual systems in mental imagery: Dissociation of “What” and “Where” in imagery disorders due to bilateral posterior cerebral lesions.Neurology, 35: 1010–1018.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Ljungberg, T., Apicella, P., and Schultz, W. (1992). Responses of monkey dopamine neurons during learning of behavioral reactions.Journal of Neurophysiology, 67: 145–163.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Mishkin, M. (1990). Vision, memory, and the temporal lobe: Summary and Perspective. In (Ed.),Vision, Memory, and the Temporal Lobe. New York: Elsevier Science Publishing Co.Google Scholar
  11. Penfield, W., and Jasper, H. (1954).Epilepsy and the Functional Anatomy of the Human Brain. Boston: Little, Brown and Company.Google Scholar
  12. Penfield, W., and Perot, P. (1963). The brain’s record of auditory and visual experience. A final summary and discussion.Brain, 86: 595–694.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Penfield, W., and Rasmussen, T. (1968).The Cerebral Cortex of Man. New York: Hafner Publishing Co.Google Scholar
  14. Penfield, W., and Roberts, L. (1959).Speech and Brain-Mechanisms. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Romo, R., and Schultz, W. (1990). Dopamine neurons of the monkey midbrain: Contingencies of responses to active touch during self-initiated arm movements.Journal of Neurophysiology, 63: 592–606.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Schultz, W., and Romo, R. (1990). Dopamine neurons of the monkey midbrain: Contingencies of responses to stimuli elicting immediate behavioral reactions.Journal of Neurophysiology, 63: 607–624.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Schultz, W., and Romo, R. (1988). Neuronal activity in the monkey striatum during the initiation of movements.Experimental Brain Research, 71: 431–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Schultz, W., Romo, R., Scarnati, E., Studer, A., Jonsson, G., and Sundstrom, E. (1989). Neural mechanisms in the basal ganglia related to the initiation of movements. In A. Crossman and M. Sambrook (Ed.),Neural Mechanisms in Disorders of Movement. London: John Libbey.Google Scholar
  19. Sommerhoff, G., and MacDorman, K. (1994). An account of consciousness in physical and functional terms: A target for research in the neurosciences.Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 29: 151–181.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Stevens, J. (1973). An anatomy of schizophrenia?Archives of General Psychiatry, 29: 177–189.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Terzian, H., and Dalle Ore, G. (1955). Syndrome of Kluver and Bucy reproduced in man by bilateral removal of the temporal lobes.Neurology, 5: 373–380.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Turner, B. (1993). Does the limbic system affect striatal function via the nigrostriatal pathway?Society for Neuroscience (Abstract), 19: 1475.Google Scholar
  23. Turner, B. (Submitted, 1994). Psychopathologies of a limbic-striatal pathway: A research model.Google Scholar
  24. Turner, B.H., Mishkin, M., and Knapp, M. (1980). Organization of the amygdalopetal projections from modality-specific cortical association areas in the monkey.Journal of Comparative Neurology, 191: 515–543.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Ungerleider, L.G., and Haxby, J.V. (1994). “What” and “where” in the human brain.Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4: 157–165.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Yates, F. (1966).The Art of Memory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Transaction Publishers 1995

Authors and Affiliations

  • Blair H. Turner
    • 1
  • Margaret E. Knapp
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Anatomy, College of MedicineHoward UniversityWashington, D.C.
  2. 2.Research and Evaluation AssociatesKensington

Personalised recommendations