Advertisement

Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science

, Volume 29, Issue 4, pp 415–422 | Cite as

Effects of stress on defensive aggression and dominance in a water competition test

  • Aldo Lucion
  • Wolfgang H. Vogel
Papers

Abstract

Water-deprived rats in a pair competing for a single source of water quickly establish a firm relationship during which one rat drinks consistently more (dominant) than the other (submissive) animal. This relationship is formed during the first competition and is very stable during subsequent tests. Exposure of dominant rats to a severe (18 hrs immobilization), but not a mild (2 hrs immobilization), stressor reduced markedly aggressive behavior and inverted transiently the dominant submissive relationship of the pairs. Exposure of submissive rats to the severe stressor resulted in only minor reductions of aggressive behavior in these animals. Prestress anxiety predicted stress effects in the dominant animals in that high-anxious animals lost more dominant behavior and weight during stress as compared with low-anxious rats. Thus, severe stress can transiently reduce dominant but not submissive behavior during water competition and high-anxious rats are more prone to lose their aggressive behavior.

Keywords

Aggressive Behavior Severe Stressor Restraint Stress Inescapable Shock Competitive Behavior 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anisman, H. and Zacharko, R.M. (1982). Depression: The predisposition influence of stress.Behavioral Brain Sciences, 5, 89–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baenninger, L.P. (1970). Social dominance orders in the rat: “Spontaneous” food and water competition.Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 71, 202–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Benus, R.F., Bohus, B., Koolhaas, J.M., and van Oortmerssen, G.A. (1990). Behavioral strategies of aggressive and non-aggressive male mice in response to inescapable shock.Behavioral Brain Sciences, 21, 127–141.Google Scholar
  4. Drew, D.R. and Dickey, G.L. (1977). Observational and competitive measures of dominance in rats.The Psychological Record, 2, 331–338.Google Scholar
  5. Drugan, R.C., Skolnick, P., Paul, S.M., and Crawley, J.N. (1989). A pretest procedure reliably predicts performance in two animal models of inescapable stress.Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior, 33, 649–654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Gray, J.A. (1987).The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  7. Landau, S.F. and Raveh, A. (1987). Stress factors, social support, and violence in Israeli society: A quantitative analysis.Aggressive Behavior, 13, 67–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Louch, C.D. and Higginbotham, M. (1967). The relation between social rank and plasma corticosterone levels in mice.General Comparative Endocrinology, 8, 441–444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Maestripieri, D., Badiani, A., and Puglisi-Allegre, S. (1991). Prepartal chronic stress increases anxiety and decreases aggression in lactating female mice.Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, 663–668.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Maier, S.F. and Seligman, M.E.P. (1976). Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence.Journal of Experimental Psychology (Gen.), 105, 3–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Moskin, M.P., Potopov, M.A., Frolova, O.F. and Evsikov, V.I. (1993). Changes in aggressive behavior, thermoregulation, and endocrine responses in mice under cold exposure.Physiology and Behavior, 53, 535–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Posner, I. and Conway, K.M. (1981). Arousal of sexual and aggressive contact by exposure to stressors in albino rats.The Journal of General Psychology, 104, 145–151.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Raab, A., Dantzer, R., Michaud, B., Mormede, P., Taghzouti, K., Simon, H., and Le Moal, M. (1986). Behavioral physiological and immunological consequences of social status and aggression in chronically coexisting resident-intruder dyads of male rats.Physiology and Behavior, 36, 223–228.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Rapaport, P.M. and Maier, S.F. (1987). Inescapable shock and food-competition dominance in rats.Animal Learning and Behavior, 6, 160–165.Google Scholar
  15. Sachser, N. and Lick, C. (1989). Social stress in guinea pigs.Physiology and Behavior, 46, 137–144.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Sachser, N. and Lick, C. (1991). Social experience, behavior, and stress in guinea pigs.Physiology and Behavior, 50, 83–90.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Seligman, M.E.P. and Beagley, G. (1975). Learned helplessness in the rat.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 534–541.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Weiss, J.M., Goodman, P.A., Losito, B.G., Corrigan, S., Charry, J.M., and Bailey, W.H. (1981). Behavioral depression produced by an uncontrollable stressor: Relationship to norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin levels in various regions of rat brain.Brain Research Reviews, 3, 167–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Williams, J.L. (1982). Influence of shock controllability by dominant rats on subsequent attack and defensive behaviors toward colony intruders.Animal Learning Behavior, 10, 305–313.Google Scholar
  20. Williams, J.L. and Lierle, D.M. (1986). Effects of stress controllability, immunization, and therapy on the subsequent defeat of colony intruders.Animal Learning Behavior, 14, 305–314.Google Scholar
  21. Williams, J.L. (1987). Effects of shock controllability on alpha male aggression and defense, defeat of intruders, and defensive burying.The Psychological Record, 37, 369–380.Google Scholar
  22. Zebroska-Lupina, I., Ossowska, G., and Klenk-Majewska, B. (1991). Chronic stress reduces fighting behavior of rats: The effect of antidepressants.Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 39, 293–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer 1994

Authors and Affiliations

  • Aldo Lucion
    • 1
  • Wolfgang H. Vogel
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Physiology, Institute of Biological SciencesFederal University of Rio Grande do SulPono AlegreBrazil
  2. 2.Department of Pharmacology and Department of Psychiatry and Human BehaviorJefferson Medical College of Thomas Jefferson UniversityPhiladelphiaUSA

Personalised recommendations