Research on cognitive load theory and its design implications for e-learning

Special Issue

Abstract

This introduction to the special issue provides a context for the contributing articles. for readers who are not familiar with cognitive load theory (CLT), it provides a very brief description of assumptions regarding memory systems and learning processes, different types of cognitive load (intrinsic, extraneous, and germane), and design implications. Whereas traditional CLT research focused on instructional methods to decrease extraneous cognitive load that is not directly relevant for learning, contributions to this special issue represent wider perspectives that reflect new developments in CLT. These articles have been organized into three categories: (a) methods to decrease intrinsic cognitive load, and deal with high-element interactivity materials, (b) methods to increase germane cognitive load that is directly relevant for learning, and (c) methods to deal with differences in learner's individual levels of expertise and expertise development. To conclude, design implications for (adaptive) e-learning are discussed.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertixe reversal effect.Educational Psychologist, 38, 23–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Kirschner, P. A. (Ed.). (2002). Cognitive load theory.Learning and Instruction,12(1), Whole Issue.Google Scholar
  3. Mayer, R. E. (2001).Multimedia learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Miller, G. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information.Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (Eds.). (2003). Cognitive load theory.Educational Psychologist,38(1), Whole Issue.Google Scholar
  6. Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (Eds.). (2004). Cognitive load theory.Instructional Science,32(1–2), Whole Issue.Google Scholar
  7. Penney, C. G. (1989). Modality effects and the structure of short term verbal memory. Memory and Cognition,17, 398–422.Google Scholar
  8. Sweller, J. (2004). Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture.Instructional Science, 32, 9–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design.Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. van Merriënboer, J. J. G., Kirschner, P. A., & Kester, L. (2003). Taking the load off a learner's mind: Instructional design for complex learning.Educational Psychologist, 38, 5–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions.Educational Psychology Review, 17(2), 147–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Educational Communications and Technology 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Educational Technology Expertise CenterOpen University of The NetherlandsHeerlenThe Netherlands
  2. 2.School of Education at the University of New South WalesAustralia

Personalised recommendations