Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 113, Issue 2, pp 243–248 | Cite as

Automatic control of postural sway by visual motion parallax

Research Article

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to establish whether visual motion parallax participates in the control of postural sway. Body sway was measured in ten normal subjects by photoelectric recordings of head movements and by force-plate posturography. Subjects viewed a visual display (“background”), which briefly moved (2 s) along the y (horizontal) axis, under three different conditions: (1) direct fixation of the background, (2) fixation of a stationary window frame in the foreground, and (3) fixation of the background in the presence of the window in the foreground (“through the window”). In response to background fixation, subjects swayed in the same direction as stimulus motion, but during foreground (window) fixation they swayed in the opposite direction. The earlier forces observed on the force platform occurred at circa 250 ms in both conditions. The results show that motion parallax generates postural responses. The direction of these parallax-evoked postural responses — opposite to other visually evoked postural responses reported so far — is appropriate for stabilizating posture in natural circumstances. The findings show that motion parallax is an important source of self-motion information and that this information participates in the process of automatic postural control. In the “fixating through the window” condition, which does not mimic visual conditions induced by body sway, no consistent postural responses were elicited. This implies that postural reactions elicited by visual motion are not rigid responses to optokinetic stimulation but responses to visual stimuli signalling self-motion.

Key words

Visual motion Parallax Posture Balance Spatial orientation Human 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bergin PS, Bronstein AM, Murray NMF, Sancovic S, Zeppenfeld K (1995) Bodysway and vibration perception thresholds in normal ageing and in patients with polyneuropathology. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 58:335–340PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bles W, Kapteyn TS, Brandt T, Arnold F (1980) The mechanism of physiological height vertigo. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 89:534–540Google Scholar
  3. Bronstein AM (1986) Suppression of visually evoked postural responses. Exp Brain Res 63:655–658PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bronstein AM (1994) Visually induced body sway in patients with visual vertigo. In: Taguchi K, Igarashi M, Mori S (eds) Vestibular and neural front. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam, pp 569–572Google Scholar
  5. Diener HC, Dichgans J, Bacher M, Gompf B (1984) Quantification of postural sway in normals and patients with cerebellar diseases. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 57:134–142PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Dichgans J, Brandt T (1978) Visual-vestibular interaction: effects of self-motion perception and postural control. In: Held R, Leibowitz HW, Teuber H-L (eds) Perception. (Handbook of sensory physiology, vol III) Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 755–804Google Scholar
  7. Ellard CG, Goodale MA, MacLaren Scorfield D, Lawrence C (1986) Visual cortical lesions abolish the use of motion parallax in the Mongolian gerbil. Exp Brain Res 64:599–602PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Keshner A, Allum JHJ, Pfaltz CR (1987) Postural coactivation and adaption in the sway stabilishing responses of normals and patients with bilateral vestibular deficit. Exp Brain Res 69:77–92PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Lee DN, Lishman JR (1975) Visual proprioceptive control of stance. J Hum Mov Stud 1:87–95Google Scholar
  10. Lestienne F, Schoechting JF, Berthoz A (1977) Postural readjustments induced by linear motion of visual scenes. Exp Brain Res 28:363–384PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Mauritz KH, Dietz V (1980) Characteristics of postural instability induced by ischemic blocking of leg afferents. Exp Brain Res 38:117–119PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Paulus WM, Straube A, Brandt T (1984) Visual stabilisation of posture. Physiological stimulus characteristics and clinical aspects. Brain 107:1143–1163PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Pola J, Wyatt HJ, Lustgarten M (1995) Visual fixation of a target and suppression of optokinetic nystagmus: effects of varying target feedback. Vision Res 35:1079–1087PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Rogers BJ (1993) Motion Parallax. In: Miles FA, Wallman J (eds), Visual motion and its role in the stabilization of gaze. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam, pp 119–137Google Scholar
  15. Roy J-P, Wurtz RH (1990) The role of disparity-sensitive cortical neurons in signalling the direction of self-motion. Nature 348:160–162PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Roy J-P, Komatsu H, Wurtz RH (1992) Disparity sensetivity of neurons in monkey extrastriate area MST. J Neurosci 12(7): 2478–2492PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Sobel EC (1990) The locust's use of motion parallax to measure distance. J Comp Physiol [A] 167:579–588Google Scholar
  18. Wallach H (1959) Perception of motion. Sci Am 201:56–62PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Warren WH, Hannon DJ (1990) Eye movements and optical flow. J Opt Soc Am 7:160–169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Wolsley CJ, Sakellari V, Bronstein AM (1996) Reorientation of visually induced postural responses by different eye-in-orbit and head-on-trunk angular positions. Exp Brain Res 111:283–288PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Woollacott MH (1983) Effects of ethanol on postural adjustments in humans. Exp Neurol 80:55–68PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1997

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.MRC Human Movement and Balance Unit, Institute of NeurologyNational Hospital for Neurology and NeurosurgeryLondonUK

Personalised recommendations