Primates

, Volume 38, Issue 1, pp 57–68 | Cite as

Huddling relationships in night sleeping groups among wild Japanese macaques in Kinkazan Island during winter

  • Hiroyuki Takahashi
Article

Abstract

Huddling groups at sleeping sites, and allogrooming and proximity in the daytime during winter, were examined in a wild Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata) troop on Kinkazan Island in the non-snowy district of northern Japan. All sleeping groups, defined as a cluster in which individuals huddle at sleeping sites, were formed on the ground. Their sizes tended to increase when the temperature was lower. The number of adults with mutual physical contact in sleeping groups increased when the size of sleeping groups increased. These results suggest that the physiological function of huddling is protection from low temperatures, and that macaques select the ground as sleeping sites to form large sized groups. Huddling was performed most frequently among kin dyads. Non-related dyads which appeared to be affiliative in the daytime also huddled frequently at sleeping sites. Even non-related dyads which showed affiliative behavior less frequently in the daytime exhibited huddling, at night, however, they did so less often than those of kin dyads and affiliated dyads. It appears that huddling at night by pairs that did not normally affiliate in the daytime was made possible by the increased tolerance of individuals responding to colder temperatures at night in winter. Furthermore, huddling, grooming, and proximity were exhibited at greater frequency between kin dyads, and between high-ranking males and specific females of kin groups, although the dyads of individuals older than 15 years often were involved only in huddling. These results suggest that two types of social bonds exist at sleeping sites in winter. One is the social bond common to both the daytime and nighttime, the other is peculiar to nighttime. Consequently, the social function of huddling is that, troop integration might increase at sleeping sites in winter as close social relationships among adults are extended more widely than those in daytime.

Key Words

Macaca fuscata Huddling Sleeping group Winter Cool temperature zone 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Altmann, J. 1980.Baboon Mothers and Infants. Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, J. R. 1984. Ethology and ecology of sleep in monkeys and apes. In:Advances in the Study of Behavior, Vol. 14,Lehrman,Daniel S. (ed.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 165–229.Google Scholar
  3. Anderson, J. R.;McGrew, W. C. 1984. Guinea baboon (Papio papio) at a sleeping site.Amer. J. Primatol., 6: 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Ashizawa, S. 1977. Japanese monkeys in the Shimokita Peninsula.The Nihonzaru, 3: 11–22. (in Japanese)Google Scholar
  5. Dunbar, R. I. M.;Nathan, M. F. 1972. Social organization of the Guinea baboon,Papio papio.Folia Primatol., 17: 321–334.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Gartlan, J. S.;Brain, C. K. 1968. Ecology and social variability inCercopithecus aethiops andCercopithecus mitis. In:Primates: Studies in Adaptation and Variability,Jay,P. C. (ed.), Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York, pp. 253–292.Google Scholar
  7. Gautier-Hion, A. 1971. L'Ecologie du talapoin du Gabon.Terre et la Vie, 25: 427–490.Google Scholar
  8. Izawa, K. 1983. The ecological study of wild Japanese monkeys living in Kinkazan Island, Miyagi Prefecture: a preliminary report.Bull. Miyagi Univ. Educ., 18: 24–46. (in Japanese with English summary)Google Scholar
  9. Izawa, K. 1992. A brief history of the ecological studies on the Japanese monkeys in Kinkazan.Miyagiken no Nihonzaru, 5: 1–2. (in Japanese)Google Scholar
  10. Maruhashi, T. 1979. Natural life of the Yakuzaru (Macaca fuscata yakui): group movement and subgrouping.Monkey, 168: 6–15. (in Japanese)Google Scholar
  11. Mori, A. 1979. Sexual difference in the tendencies to gather in night aggregations and grouping in the daytime in Japanese monkeys of the Koshima troop. In:The Differences of the Role of Sex in the Societies of Japanese Monkeys in Different Regional Population, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto Univ., Inuyama, Japan, pp. 41–49. (in Japanese)Google Scholar
  12. Nakagawa, N. 1989a. Feeding strategies of Japanese monkeys against deterioration of habitat quality.Primates, 30: 1–16.Google Scholar
  13. Nakagawa, N. 1989b. Bioenergetics of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) on Kinkazan Island during winter.Primates, 30: 441–460.Google Scholar
  14. Napier, J. R.;Napier, P. H. 1985.The Natural History of the Primates. British Museum (Natural History) and Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  15. Sato, S. 1988. Monkeys of Kinkazan A-troop.Miyagiken no Nihonzaru, 3: 6–29. (in Japanese)Google Scholar
  16. Vessey, S. H. 1973. Night observation of free-ranging rhesus monkeys.Amer. J. Phys. Anthropol., 38: 613–620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Wada, K. 1964. Some observations on the life of monkeys in a snowy district of Japan.Physiol. Ecol., 12 (1, 2): 151–174. (in Japanese with English summary)Google Scholar
  18. Wada, K.;Ichiki, Y. 1980. Seasonal habitat utilization of the Japanese monkeys in the Shiga Heights. In:Cold Adaptation of Japanese Monkeys, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto Univ., Inuyama, Japan, pp. 5–23. (in Japanese)Google Scholar
  19. Wada, K.;Tokida, E. 1985. Sleeping site cluster of Japanese monkeys in Shiga A1 troop: about inter-individual relationships. In:The Studies of Spatial Distribution of the Japanese Monkey Troops in Shiga Heights, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto Univ., Inuyama, Japan, pp. 2–10. (in Japanese)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Japan Monkey Centre 1997

Authors and Affiliations

  • Hiroyuki Takahashi
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Zoology, Faculty of ScienceKyoto UniversityKyotoJapan

Personalised recommendations