Primates

, Volume 31, Issue 3, pp 375–383 | Cite as

Tool use by wild cebus monkeys at Santa Rosa National Park, Costa Rica

  • Suzanne Chevalier-Skolnikoff
Article

Abstract

Although wild cebus monkeys have been observed to use tools, this behavior has been reported only rarely. No one has systematically examined tool use in wildCebus, and it is not known how prevalent tool use is in the species' natural repertoire. During 300 hr of observation on 21 wild capuchins (Cebus capucinus imitator) at Santa Rosa National Park in Costa Rica, 31 incidents of tool use, including eight different types of tool-use behavior, were observed. These observations indicate that tool use is a notable behavior pattern in this troop. Considering these incidents of tool use in conjunction with other reports on complex food-getting and preparation behavior byCebus suggests that tool use is a manifestation ofCebus' high behavioral adaptability. Since onlyCebus and the great apes (especially chimpanzees) have been observed to show such a diverse tool-use repertoire, to use tools so frequently, or to show such complex food-getting behavior in the wild, these observations also support the notion thatCebus and the great apes have followed a parallel evolutionary development of tool-using capacity.

Key Words

Aggression Cebus Monkeys Parallel evolution Sensorimotor intelligence Tool use 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Antinucci, F. &E. Visalberghi, 1986. Tool use inCebus apella: A case study.Int. J. Primatol., 7: 351–363.Google Scholar
  2. Beck, B., 1980.Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals. Garland STPM Press, New York.Google Scholar
  3. Bierens de Haan, J. A., 1931. Werkzeuggebrauch und Werkzeugherstellung bie einem niederen Affen (Cebus hypoleucus Humb.).Z. vergl. Physiol., 13: 639–695.Google Scholar
  4. Boesch, C. &H. Boesch, 1983. Optimisation of nut-cracking with natural hammers by wild chimpanzees.Behaviour, 83: 265–286.Google Scholar
  5. ———— & ————, 1984. The nut-cracking behaviour and its nutritional importance in wild chimpanzees in the Tai National Park, Ivory Coast.Int. J. Primatol., 5: 323.Google Scholar
  6. Boinski, S., 1988. Use of a club by a wild white-faced capuchin (Cebus capucinus) to attack a venomous snake (Bothrops asper).Amer. J. Primatol., 14: 177–179.Google Scholar
  7. Boulenger, E. G., 1936.Apes and Monkeys. George G. Harrap, London.Google Scholar
  8. Chapman, C. A., 1986. Boa constrictor predation and group response in white-faced cebus monkeys.Biotropica, 18(2): 171–172.Google Scholar
  9. Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S., 1978. Intelligence in wild cebus monkeys and its implications for the evolution of tool use. Paper presented at the 77th Annual Meeting of the American Anthropological Association, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
  10. ----, 1984. Intelligence, manipulation and tool use in wild cebus monkeys. Paper presented at the Xth Congress of the International Primatological Society, Nairobi, Kenya.Google Scholar
  11. ————, 1989. Spontaneous tool use and sensorimotor intelligence inCebus compared with other monkeys and apes.Behav. Brain Sci., 12(3): 561–627.Google Scholar
  12. Cooper, L. R. &H. F. Harlow, 1961. Note on cebus monkey's use of a stick as a weapon.Psychol. Rep., 8: 418.Google Scholar
  13. Costello, M., 1987. Tool use and manufacture in manipulanda-deprived capuchins (Cebus apella): Ontogenetic and phylogenetic issues.Amer. J. Primatol., 12: 337.Google Scholar
  14. Defler, T. R., 1979. On the ecology and behavior ofCebus albifrons in Eastern Colombia: I. Ecology.Primates, 20: 475–490.Google Scholar
  15. Freese, C., 1976a. CensusingAlouatta palliata, Ateles geoffroyi, andCebus capucinus in the Costa Rican dry forest. In:Neotropical Primates: Field Studies and Conservation,R. W. Thorington,Jr. &P. G. Heltne (eds.), National Academy of Sciences, Washington, D.C., pp. 4–9.Google Scholar
  16. ————, 1976b. Food habits of white-faced capuchinsCebus capucinus L. (Primates: Cebidae) in Santa Rosa National Park, Costa Rica.Brenesia, 10/11: 43–56.Google Scholar
  17. Galdikas, B. M. F., 1982. Orangutan tool use at Tanjung Puting Reserve, Central Indonesian Borneo.J. Human Evol., 11: 19–33.Google Scholar
  18. Goodall, J., 1986.The Chimpanzees of Gombe. Belknap Press of Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  19. Hall, K. R. L., 1963. Tool-using performances as indicators of behavioral adaptability.Cur. Anthropol., 4(5): 479–494.Google Scholar
  20. Harlow, H. F., 1951. Primate learning. In:Comparative Psychology (3rd ed.),C. P. Stone (ed.), Prentice-Hall, New York, pp. 183–238.Google Scholar
  21. Hernandez-Camacho, J. &R. W. Cooper, 1976. The nonhuman primates of Colombia. In:Neotropical Primates: Field Studies and Conservation,R. W. Thorington,Jr. &P. G. P. Heltne (eds.), National Academy of Sciences, Washington, D.C., pp. 35–69.Google Scholar
  22. Hill, W. C. O., 1960.Primates. Vol. IV. Cebidae Part A. Edinburgh Univ. Press, Edinburgh.Google Scholar
  23. Izawa, K., 1978. Frog-eating behavior of wild black-capped capuchin (Cebus apella).Primates, 19: 633–642.Google Scholar
  24. ————, 1979. Foods and feeding behavior of wild black-capped capuchin (Cebus apella).Primates, 20: 57–76.Google Scholar
  25. ———— &A. Mizuno, 1977. Palm-fruit cracking behavior of wild black-capped capuchin (Cebus apella).Primates, 18: 773–792.Google Scholar
  26. Kaufmann, J., 1962. Ecology and social behavior of the coati (Nasua narica) on Barro Colorado Island, Panama.Univ. Calif. Publ. Zool., 60: 95–222.Google Scholar
  27. Klein, L. L., 1974. Agonistic behavior in neotropical primates. In:Primate Aggression, Territoriality, and Xenophobia,R. L. Holloway (ed.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 77–122.Google Scholar
  28. Klüver, H., 1933.Behavior Mechanisms in Monkeys. Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  29. ————, 1937. Re-examination of implement-using behavior in a cebus monkey after an interval of three years.Acta Psychol., 2: 347–397.Google Scholar
  30. Kortlandt, A. &M. Kooij, 1963. Protohominid behaviour in primates.Symp. Zool. Soc. London, 10: 61–88.Google Scholar
  31. Lancaster, J., 1968. On the evolution of tool-using behavior.Amer. Anthropol., 70: 55–66.Google Scholar
  32. Lethmate, J., 1982. Tool-using skills of orang-utans.J. Human Evol., 11: 49–64.Google Scholar
  33. McBeath, N. M. &W. C. McGrew, 1982. Tools used by wild chimpanzees to obtain termites at Mt. Assirik, Senegal: The influence of habitat.J. Human Evol., 11: 65–72.Google Scholar
  34. McGrew, W. C., 1977. Socialization and object manipulation of wild chimpanzees. In:Primate Bio-social Development: Biological, Social, and Ecological Determinants,S. Chevalier-Skolnikoff &F. E. Poirier (eds.), Garland STPM Press, New York, pp. 261–288.Google Scholar
  35. ————,C. E. G. Tutin, &P. J. Baldwin, 1979. Chimpanzees, tools, and termites: Cross-cultural comparisons of Senegal, Tanzania, and Rio Muni.Man, 14: 185–214.Google Scholar
  36. Nishida, T. &M. Hiraiwa, 1982. Natural history of a tool-using behavior by wild chimpanzees in feeding upon wood-boring ants.J. Human Evol., 11: 73–99.Google Scholar
  37. Nolte, A., 1958. Beobachtungen uber das Instinktverhalten von Kapuzineraffen (Cebus apella L.) in der Gefangenschaft.Behaviour, 12(3): 183–207.Google Scholar
  38. Oppenheimer, J. R., 1968. Behavior and ecology of the whitefaced monkey,Cebus capucinus, on Barro Colorado Island. C. Z. Univ. Microfilms, Inc., Ann Arbor.Google Scholar
  39. Parker, S. T. &K. R. Gibson, 1977. Object manipulation, tool use and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in cebus monkeys and great apes.J. Human Evol., 6: 623–641.Google Scholar
  40. ————, 1979. A developmental model for the evolution of language and intelligence in early hominids.Behav. Brain Sci., 2: 367–408.Google Scholar
  41. Piaget, J., 1952.The Origins of Intelligence in Children (Translated byM. Cook). International Universities Press, Inc., New York.Google Scholar
  42. Romanes, G. J., 1882.Animal Intelligence (2nd ed.). Kegan Paul, Trench, London.Google Scholar
  43. Schaller, G. B., 1963.The Mountain Gorilla. Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  44. Struhsaker, T. T. &L. Leland, 1977. Palm-nut smashing byCebus a. apella in Colombia.Biotropica, 9: 124–126.Google Scholar
  45. Sugiyama, Y. &J. Koman, 1979. Tool-using and -making behavior in wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea.Primates, 20: 513–524.Google Scholar
  46. Terborgh, J., 1983.Five New World Primates: A Study in Comparative Ecology, Princeton Univ. Press, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  47. Thorington, R. W. Jr., 1967. Feeding and activity ofCebus andSaimiri in a Colombian forest. In:Neue Ergebnisse der Primatologie,D. Starck,R. Schneider, &H. J. Kuhn (eds.), Gustav Fischer Verlag, Stuttgart, pp. 180–184.Google Scholar
  48. Tobias, P. V., 1965.Australopithecus, Homo habilis, tool-using and tool-making.S. Afr. Archaeol. Bull., 20: 167–192.Google Scholar
  49. van Lawick-Goodall, J., 1968. The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve.Anim. Behav. Monogr., 1: 161–311.Google Scholar
  50. Vevers, G. M. &J. S. Weiner, 1963. Use of a tool by a captive capuchin monkey (Cebus apella).Symp. Zool. Soc. London, 10: 115–118.Google Scholar
  51. Visalberghi, E., 1987. Acquisition of nut-cracking behaviour by two capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).Folia Primatol., 49: 168–181.Google Scholar
  52. Warden, C. J., A. M. Koch, &H. A. Fjeld, 1940. Instrumentation in cebus and rhesus monkeys.J. Genet. Psychol., 56: 297–310.Google Scholar
  53. Washburn, S. L., 1960. Tools and human evolution.Sci. Amer., 203: 63–75.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Westergaard, G. C. &D. M. Fragaszy, 1985. Effects of manipulatable objects on the activity of captive capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).Zoo Biol., 4: 317–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. ———— & ————, 1987. The manufacture and use of tools by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).J. Comp. Psychol., 101(2): 159–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Japan Monkey Centre 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Suzanne Chevalier-Skolnikoff
    • 1
  1. 1.California Primate Research CenterUniversity of CaliforniaDavisU.S.A.

Personalised recommendations