Dynamic visual support for story comprehension and mental model building by young, at-risk children

  • Diana L. M. Sharp
  • John D. Bransford
  • Susan R. Goldman
  • Victoria J. Risko
  • Charles K. Kinzer
  • Nancy J. Vye
Research

Abstract

Multimedia technology allows precise coordination of linguistic and visual information and may provide teachers with tools for enhancing literacy foundations in children–especially those who might otherwise be at-risk for school failure. This research explores the hypothesis that a multimedia environment with dynamic visual support facilitates language comprehension when children listen to short stories. Kindergarten children heard stories in three conditions: Helpful video, in which dynamic, silent video accompanied the beginning of stories; No video, in which children only heard the stories; and Minimal video, in which static images of characters and places accompanied the beginning of stories. In all conditions, the ending of the story was presented without visual support. Overall, the pattern of results suggests that dynamic visual support can provide a framework for understanding and remembering linguistic information.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anderson, A.B., & Stokes, S.J. (1984). Social and institutional influences on the development and practice of literacy. In H. Goelman, A. Oberg, & F. Smith (Eds.),Awakening to literacy (pp. 24–38). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.Google Scholar
  2. Beagles-Roos, J., & Gat, I. (1983). Specific impact of radio and television on children's story comprehension.Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(1), 128–137.Google Scholar
  3. Bransford, J.D. & Johnson, M.K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 717–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bus, A.G., & van Ijzendoorn, M.H. (1988). Mother-child interactions, attachment, and emergent literacy: A cross-sectional study.Child Development, 59, 1262–1272.Google Scholar
  5. Crowder, R.B., & Wagner, R.K. (1992).The psychology of reading: An introduction (2nd ed.), New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Curtis (1980). Development of components of reading skill.Journal of Educational Psychology, 72(5), 656–669.Google Scholar
  7. Digdon, N., Pressley, M., & Levin, J.R. (1985). Preschoolers' learning when pictures do not tell the whole story.Educational Communication and Technology Journal, 33(2), 139–145.Google Scholar
  8. Feitelson, D., Kita, B., & Goldstein, Z. (1986). Effects of listening to series stories on first graders' comprehension and use of language.Research in the Teaching of English, 20(4), 339–356.Google Scholar
  9. Gernsbacher, M.A. (1990).Language comprehension as structure building. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Gibbons, J., Anderson, D.R., Smith, R., Field, D.E., & Fischer, C. (1986). Young children's recall and reconstruction of audio and audiovisual narratives.Child Development, 57, 1014–1023.Google Scholar
  11. Gibson, J.J. (1966).The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Company.Google Scholar
  12. Glenberg, A.M., & Langston, W.E. (1992). Comprehension of illustrated text: Pictures help to build mental models.Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 129–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Glenberg, A.M., Meyer, M., & Lindem, K. (1987). Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension.Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 69–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Guttman, J., Levin, J.R., & Pressley, M. (1977). Pictures, partial pictures, and young children's oral prose learning.Journal of Educational Psychology, 69(5), 473–480.Google Scholar
  15. Hayes, D.S., Kelly, S.B., & Mandel, M. (1986). Media differences in children's story synopses: Radio and television contrasted.Journal of Educational Psychology, 78(5), 341–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Health (1983).Ways with words (1st ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1983).Mental models. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Juel, C. (1992). Longitudinal research on learning to read and write with at-risk students. In M. J. Dreher & W. H. Slater (Eds.),Elementary school literacy: Critical issues (pp. 73–99). Norwood, MA: Christopher-Gordon Publishers, Inc.Google Scholar
  19. Kamil, M. (1991).A proposal for the National Reading Research Center. College of Education, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH.Google Scholar
  20. Kirsch, I., & Jungblut, A. (1986).Literacy: Profiles of America's young adults. (ETS No. 16-Pl-02). National Assessment of Educational Progress.Google Scholar
  21. McNamara, T.P., Miller, D.L., & Bransford, J.D. (1991). Mental models and narrative comprehension. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.),Handbook of reading research (Vol. II). (pp. 490–511). New York: Longman.Google Scholar
  22. Meringoff, L.K. (1980). Influence of the medium on children's story apprehension.Journal of Educational Psychology, 72(2), 240–249.Google Scholar
  23. Morrow, D.G., Bower, G.H., & Greenspan, S. (1989). Updating situation models during narrative comprehension.Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 292–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Morrow, L.M. (1985). Retelling stories: A strategy for improving young children's comprehension, concept of story structure, and oral language complexity.The Elementary School Journal, 85, 647–661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Morrow, L.M. (1986). Effects of structural guidance in story retelling of children's dictation of original stories.Journal of Reading Behavior, 18(2), 135–152.Google Scholar
  26. Morrow, L.M. (1988). Young children's responses to one-to-one readings in school settings.Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 89–107.Google Scholar
  27. Morrow, L.M. (1990). Small group story readings: The effects on children's comprehension and responses to literature.Reading Research and Instruction, 29(4), 1–17.Google Scholar
  28. Ninio, A. (1980). Picture-book reading in motherinfant dyads belonging to two subgroups in Israel.Child Development, 51, 587–590.Google Scholar
  29. Pezdek, K., Lehrer, A., & Simon, S. (1984). The relationship between reading and cognitive processing of television and radio.Child Development, 55, 2072–2082.Google Scholar
  30. Pressley, M., Cariglia-Bull, T., Deane, S., & Schneider, W. (1987). Short-term memory, verbal competence, and age as predictors of imagery instructional effectiveness.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 43, 194–211.Google Scholar
  31. Purcell-Gates, V. (1989). Written language knowledge held by low-SES, inner city children entering kindergarten. In S. McCormick & J. Zutrell (Eds.),Cognitive and social perspectives for literacy research and instruction: Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Reading Conference (pp. 95–105). Chicago, IL: National Reading Conference.Google Scholar
  32. Sharp, D.L.M., & McNamara, T.P. (1990).Spatial mental models in narrative comprehension: Now you see them, now you don't. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans, LA.Google Scholar
  33. Smiley, S.S., Oakley, D.D., Worthen, D., Campione, J.C., & Brown, A.L. (1977). Recall of thematically relevant material by adolescent good and poor readers as a function of written versus oral presentation.Journal of Educational Psychology, 69(4), 381–387.Google Scholar
  34. Snow, C.E. (1983). Lieracy and language: Relationships during the preschool years.Harvard Educational Review, 53(2), 165–189.Google Scholar
  35. Snow, C.E., & Goldfield, B.A. (1983). Turn the page, please: Situation-specific language acquisition.Journal of Child Language, 10, 535–549.Google Scholar
  36. Snow, C.E., & Ninio, A. (1986). The contracts of literacy: What children learn from learning to read books. In W.H. Teale & E. Sulzby, (Eds.),Emergent literacy: Writing and reading. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  37. Teale, W.H. (1986). Home background and young children's literacy development. In W.H. Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.),Emergent literacy: Writing and reading (pp. 173–206). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  38. Wells, G. (1985). Preschool literacy-related activities and success in school. In D.R. Olson, N. Torrence, & N. Hildyard (Eds.),Literacy, language, and learning (pp. 229–255). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Yuill, N.A., & Oakhill, J. (1991).Children's problems in text comprehension: An experimental investigation. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© the Association for Educational Communications and Technology 1995

Authors and Affiliations

  • Diana L. M. Sharp
    • 1
  • John D. Bransford
    • 2
  • Susan R. Goldman
    • 3
  • Victoria J. Risko
    • 4
  • Charles K. Kinzer
    • 4
  • Nancy J. Vye
    • 1
  1. 1.Learning Technology CenterUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyLearning Technology CenterUSA
  3. 3.Department of Psychology and Human DevelopmentLearning Technology CenterUSA
  4. 4.Department of Teaching and LearningUSA

Personalised recommendations