Advertisement

Stimulus complexity and autistic children's responsivity: Assessing and training a pivotal behavior

  • John C. Burke
  • Laurie Cerniglia
Article

Abstract

Interdisciplinary research suggests that autistic children's limitations in responding to environmental stimuli may be directly related to the number of components contained in the stimuli; as the number of components increases, such children hypothetically would exhibit greater difficulties in responding. The central purpose of this experiment was to assess whether such children indeed exhibit greater difficulties in responding as the number of components contained in an environmental stimulus was increased from one to four. If the children's responsivity was a function of stimulus complexity, a second focus of this experiment was to assess the feasibility of teaching them to respond to a complex environmental stimulus containing up to four components and to determine whether the effects of the intervention would generalize to other situations involving complex structured and social stimuli. Data gathered using a multiple baseline design across behaviors and children indicate that all of the children exhibited fewer correct responses to a stimulus as the number of stimulus components was increased from one to four. The results further showed that the training program used in this investigation was effective in producing some generalized increases in the children's responses to complex structured and social stimuli. Conceptualizing autistic children's responses to complex multicomponent stimuli as a pivotal target behavior that can be operationally defined may have important implications for understanding and altering the children's responsivity and development.

Keywords

Training Program Correct Response School Psychology Great Difficulty Generalize Increase 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Allen, K. D., & Fuqua, R. W. (1985). Eliminating selective stimulus control: A comparison of two procedures for teaching mentally retarded children to respond to compound stimuli.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 55–71.Google Scholar
  2. Bagshaw, N. B. (1978).An acoustic analysis of fundamental frequency and temporal parameters of autistic children's speech. Unpublished master's thesis, University of California, Santa Barbara.Google Scholar
  3. Bandura, A. (1969).Principles of behavior modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  4. Barlow, D. H., & Hersen, M. (1984).Single case experimental designs: Strategies for studying behavioral change (2nd ed.). New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  5. Bauman, M., & Kemper, T. L. (1985). Histoanatomic observations of the brain in early infantile autism.Neurology 35, 866–874.Google Scholar
  6. Bennet, E. L., Diamond, M. C., Kretch, D., & Rosenzweig, M. R. (1964). Chemical and anatomical plasticity of the brain.Science 146, 610–619.Google Scholar
  7. Bennet, E. L., Rosenzweig, M. R., & Diamond, M. C. (1969). Rat brain: Effects of environment enrichment on wet and dry weights.Science, 163, 825–826.Google Scholar
  8. Bickel, W. K., Stella, M. E., & Etzel, B. C. (1984). A reevaluation of stimulus overselectivity: Restricted stimulus control or stimulus control hierarchies.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 137–157.Google Scholar
  9. Bryson, C. Q. (1970). Systematic identification of perceptual disabilities in autistic children.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 31, 239–246.Google Scholar
  10. Burke, J. C. (1982).Collateral effects of parent-training on the daily interactions of families with autistic children. Unpublished master's thesis, University of California, Santa Barbara.Google Scholar
  11. Burke, J. C., & Koegel, R. L. (1982, May).The relationship between stimulus overselectivity and autistic children's social responsiveness and incidental learning. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Behavioral Analysis. Milwaukee, WI.Google Scholar
  12. Damasio, A. R., & Manner, R. G. (1978). A neurological model for childhood autism.Archives of Neurology, 35, 777–786.Google Scholar
  13. Das, J. P., Kirby, J. R., & Jarman, R. F. (1979).Simultaneous and successive cognitive processes. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  14. DiSimoni, F. (1978).The Token Test for Children. Hingham, MA: Teaching Resources.Google Scholar
  15. Dunlap, G. (1984). The influence of task variation and maintenance tasks on the learning and affect of autistic children.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 41–64.Google Scholar
  16. Dunlap, G., Koegel, R. L.,& Burke, J. C. (1981). Educational implications of stimulus overselectivity in autistic children.Exceptional Education Quarterly, 2, 37–49.Google Scholar
  17. Finger, S., & Stein, D. G. (1982).Brain damage and recovery: Research and clinical perspective. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  18. Fletcher, C. E. (1976).A comparison of pitch patterns of normal and autistic children. Unpublished master's thesis, University of California, Santa Barbara.Google Scholar
  19. Foster, R., Gidan, J. J., & Stark, J. (1973).Manual of Assessment of Children's Language Comprehension. Consulting Psychological Press.Google Scholar
  20. Frankel, F., Simmons, J. Q., Fichter, M., & Freeman, B. J. (1984). Stimulus overselectivity in autistic and mentally retarded children—A research note.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25, 147–155.Google Scholar
  21. Gersten, R. M. (1978).Overselective attention vs. information processing research on the cognitive development of the autistic child. Paper presented at the annual Conference of the Canadian Society for Autistic Children, Vancouver, BC.Google Scholar
  22. Gersten, R. M. (1980). In search of the cognitive deficit in autism: Beyond the stimulus overselectivity model.Journal of Special Education, 14, 47–65.Google Scholar
  23. Gold, M. S., & Gold, J. R. (1975). Autism and attention: Theoretical considerations and a pilot study using set reaction time.Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 6, 116–123.Google Scholar
  24. Greenough, W. T. (1975). Experimental modification of the developing brain.American Scientist, 63, 59–65.Google Scholar
  25. Greenough, W. T., & Juraska, J. M. (1986). (Eds.).Developmental neuropsychobiology. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  26. Headbring, C., & Newsom, C. (1985). Visual overselectivity: A comparison of two instructional remediation procedures with autistic children.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15, 9–22.Google Scholar
  27. Hermelin, B., & Connor, N. (1964). Effects of sensory input and sensory dominance on severely-disturbed children and on subnormal controls.British Journal of Psychology, 55, 201–206.Google Scholar
  28. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact.Nervous Child, 3, 217–250.Google Scholar
  29. Kanner, L. (1944). Early infantile autism.Journal of Pediatrics, 25, 211–217.Google Scholar
  30. Koegel, R. L., Burke, J. C., Kennedy, C. H., & Lanahan, K. (1987).Pivotal target behaviors in the treatment of autistic children. Poster presented at a symposium on Behavioral Pediatrics, Association for Behavior Analysis/Southern California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
  31. Koegel, R. L., & Felsenfeld, S. (1977). Sensory deprivation. In S. Gerber (Ed.),Audiometry in infancy. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
  32. Koegel, R. L., Russo, D. C., & Rincover, A. (1977). Assessing and training teachers in the generalized use of behavior modification with autistic children.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 197–205.Google Scholar
  33. Koegel, R. L., & Schreibman, L. (1977). Teaching autistic children to respond to simultaneously multiple cues.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 24, 299–311.Google Scholar
  34. Kovattana, P. & Kraemer, H. (1974). Response to multiple visual cues; color, size, and form by autistic children.Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4, 251–261.Google Scholar
  35. Lovaas, O. I. (1966). A program for the establishment of speech in psychotic children. In J. K. Wing (Ed.),Early childhood autism (pp. 115–144). Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  36. Lovaas, O. I., Berberich, J. P., Perloff, B. F., & Schaeffer, B. (1966). Acquisition of imitative speech in schizophrenic children.Science, 151, 705–707.Google Scholar
  37. Lovaas, O. I., & Schreibman, L. (1971). Stimulus overselectivity in autistic children in a two-stimulus situation.Behavior Research and Therapy, 9, 305–310.Google Scholar
  38. Lovaas, O. I., Schreibman, L., Koegel, R. L., & Rehm, R. (1971). Selective responding by autistic children to multiple sensory input.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 77, 211–222.Google Scholar
  39. McReynolds, L. V., & Kearns, K. P. (1983).Single-subject experimental designs in communicative disorders. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  40. Merzenich, M. M. (1985). Dynamic neocortical processes and the origins of higher brain functions.Google Scholar
  41. Mesibov, G. B., & Schaffer, B. (1986). Autism in adolescents and adults.Advances in Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 7, 313–343.Google Scholar
  42. Nelson, D. I., Gergenti, E., & Hollander, A. C. (1980). Extra prompts versus no extra prompts in self-care training of autistic children and adolescents.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10, 311–321.Google Scholar
  43. Neville, H. J. (1985). Effects of early sensory and language experience on the development of the human brain. In J. Mehler & R. Fox (Ed.),Neonate cognition: Beyond the blooming buzzing confusion (pp. 349–364). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  44. Niedermyer, E., & Lopes da Silva, F. (Eds.). (1987).Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, clinical applications, and related fields. Baltimore: Urban & Schwarzenberg.Google Scholar
  45. Noll, J. D. (1970, November).The use of the Token Test with children. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech and Hearing Association, New York.Google Scholar
  46. Ornitz, E. M. (1974). The modulation of sensory input and motor output in autistic children.Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4, 197–215.Google Scholar
  47. Prior, M. R. (1977). Conditional matching learning set performance in autistic children.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18, 183–189.Google Scholar
  48. Reynolds, B. S., Newson, C. D., & Lovaas, O. I. (1974). Auditory overselectivity in autistic children.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 2, 253–263.Google Scholar
  49. Riesen, A. H. (Ed.). (1975).The developmental neuropsychology of sensory deprivation. San Francisco: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  50. Rimland, B. (1964).Infantile autism. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  51. Ritvo, E. R., & Freeman, B. J. (1978). National Society for autistic children's definition of the syndrome of autism.Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 162–169.Google Scholar
  52. Rosenzweig, M. R., Bennett, E. L., & Diamond, M. C. (1973). Effects of differential experience on dendritic spine counts in rat cerebral cortex.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 82, 175–181.Google Scholar
  53. Schreibman, L., Charlop, M. H., & Koegel, R. L. (1982). Teaching autistic children to use extrastimulus prompts.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 475–491.Google Scholar
  54. Schreibman, L., Kohlenberg, B. S., & Britten, K. R. (1986). Differential responding to content and intonation components of a complex auditory stimulus by nonverbal and echolalic autistic children.Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 109–125.Google Scholar
  55. Schreibman, L., & Lovaas, O. I. (1973). Overselective responding to social stimuli by autistic children.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1, 152–168.Google Scholar
  56. Varni, J. W., Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R. L., & Eveerett, N. L. (1979). An analysis of observational learning in autistic and normal children.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 7, 31–43.Google Scholar
  57. Walsh, R. N., & Greenough, W. T. (1976).Environments as Therapy for Brain Dysfunction, New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  58. West, R. W., & Greenough, W. T. (1972). Effect of environmental complexity on cortical synapse of rats: Preliminary results.Behavioral Biology, 7, 279–284.Google Scholar
  59. Zubek, J. P. (1969).Sensory deprivation: Fifteen years of research. New York: Appleton.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • John C. Burke
    • 1
  • Laurie Cerniglia
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Behavioral Psychology, The Kennedy InstituteJohns Hopkins University School of MedicineBaltimore
  2. 2.University of CaliforniaSanta Barbara

Personalised recommendations