Advertisement

Journal of Adult Development

, Volume 3, Issue 1, pp 1–6 | Cite as

Starting out: The early phases of four creative careers—darwin, van Gogh, Freud, and Shaw

  • Howard E. Gruber
Article
  • 55 Downloads

Abstract

This paper introduces the four that follow it. The others deal with the phase of the creative career we call “starting out.” Of the four individuals discussed in those papers, Darwin moved rather smoothly from an early phase of field work in natural history to a later phase of theoretical biology. His work in natural history became incorporated within the theoretical enterprise, the whole move being accomplished by about the age of 30. van Gogh had a protracted early phase as a mediocre art dealer and then as a failed Christian minister to the poor, rejected and scorned by his co-religionists. At about the age of 30 he made a clear commitment to a career in art and began a long period of training, seeking help from other artists but mainly directing his own development. As with Darwin there was a certain degree of overlap of earlier and later phases, in the sense that his art displayed religious overtones for some time. Freud had brilliant early careers—as a neurologist and as a medical practitioner. He did not move decisively toward psychoanalysis until his early forties. Shaw, too, had brilliant early careers—as a radical orator, as a journalist, and as a music critic. His career as a dramatist did not begin in earnest until his forties. Three of the four produced “initial sketches” that were significant in prefiguring their later careers: Darwin'sJournal of the voyage of theBeagle, Freud'sThe Project, and Shaw's letter,Dear Dorothea. Other topics are taken up as appropriate in one or other of the four cases: the network of enterprise, the ensemble of metaphors, the belief system, and the social web. Taken together with a discussion of the uniqueness of each creative person, they give a sense of the evolving systems approach to creative work.

Key words

Darwin van Gogh Freud Shaw creativity evolving systems development 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Amram, R. (1995).The Arts and Sciences Academy. Tel Aviv, Israel: The Society for Excellence Through Education.Google Scholar
  2. Arnheim, R. (1962).The genesis of a painting: Picasso's Guernica. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bernstein, J. (1990). Review of W. Moore,Schroedinger: Life and Thought, Cambridge University Press, by J. Bernstein inNew Yorker, Nov. 5, 1990, 142–155.Google Scholar
  4. Farber, S. L. (1981).Nature vs. Nurture: A Natural Experiment. Identical Twins Reared Apart. A Reanalysis. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  5. Gruber, H. E. (1978). Darwin's “Tree of Nature” and other images of wide scope. In J. Wechsler (Ed.),On aesthetics in science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  6. Gruber, H. E. (1981).Darwin on man: A psychological study of scientific creativity (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  7. Gruber, H. E. (1985). Divergence in evolution and individuality in development. In G. Butterworth, J. Rutkowska, & M. Scaife (Eds.),Evolution and developmental psychology. Brighton, England: The Harvester Press.Google Scholar
  8. Gruber, H. E. (1986). Which way is “Up”? A developmental question. In R. A. Mines & K. S. Kitchener (Eds.),Adult cognitive development: Methods and models. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  9. Gruber, H. E. (1994). On reliving theWanderjahr: The many voyages of theBeagle.Journal of Adult Development, 1, 47–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gruber, H. E. (1995). Insight and affect in the history of science. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.),The nature of insight (pp. 397–432). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  11. Hoffmann, R. (1993). How should chemists think?Scientific American, 268, 66–73.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Lewin, K. (1935).Dynamic theory of personality. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  13. Lindauer, M. (1993). The span of creativity among long-lived historical artists.Creativity Research Journal, 8, 221–240.Google Scholar
  14. Lindauer, M. S. (1992). Creativity in aging artists. Contributions from the humanities to the psychology of old age.Creativity Research Journal, 5, 211–231.Google Scholar
  15. Schweber, S. S. (1994).QED and the men who made it: Dyson, Feynman, Schwinger, and Tomonaga. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Tulving, E. (1962). Subjective organization in free recall of unrelated words.Psychological Review, 69, 344–354.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Wallace, D. B. (1989). Stream of consciousness and reconstruction of self in Dorothy Richardson'sPilgrimage. In D. B. Wallace & H. E. Gruber (Eds.),Creative people at work: Twelve cognitive case studies (pp. 147–170). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1996

Authors and Affiliations

  • Howard E. Gruber
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Teachers CollegeColumbia UniversityNew York

Personalised recommendations