Educational Psychology Review

, Volume 7, Issue 3, pp 243–267 | Cite as

Cognition and creativity

  • Mark A. Runco
  • Ivonne Chand
Article

Abstract

Cognitive research on creativity is both traditional and innovative. It is traditional in the sense that many of the well-recognized processes, structures, and stores from mainstream cognitive psychology have been used to understand creative thinking. It is innovative because there is a need to understand processes which are not recognized unless one is specifically interested in creativity. Some of these are inherently subjective, a fact which is often disregarded by those hoping for a traditionally scientific analysis. Still, much of the interest in the cognitive sciences concerns how new constructs come into being; and anyone interested in that is in fact thinking about creativity. That is creativity. This article reviews several traditional cognitive topics, including knowledge, memory, classification, judgment, and categorization, and describes how each can influence creative thinking. It also presents an original model of creative thinking with problem finding, ideation, and judgmental processes as primary components, and knowledge and motivation as secondary (contributing but not controlling) components. Several issues are covered, including the relegation of motivation, the distinction between declarative and procedural knowledge, and the potential for knowledge to both facilitate and inhibit creative ideation.

Key Words

creativity cognition divergent thinking 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Amabile, T. M. (1990). Within you, without you: Toward a social psychology of creativity, and beyond. In Runco, M. A., and Albert, R. S. (eds.),Theories of Creativity, Sage, Newbury Park, CA.Google Scholar
  2. Amabile, T. M. (August 1993). Presented at the Meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
  3. Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., and Austin, G. A. (1962).A Study of Thinking, Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  4. Campbell, D. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes.Psychol. Rev. 67: 380–400.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Chand, I., and Runco, M. A. (1992). Problem finding skills as components in the creative process.Person. Indiv. Diff. 14: 155–162.Google Scholar
  6. Charlton, S., and Bakan, P. (1988–1989). Cognitive complexity and creativity.Imag., Cognit., Person. 8: 315–322.Google Scholar
  7. Christenson, P. R., Guilford, J. P., and Wilson, R. C. (1957). Relations of working time and instructions.J. Exp. Psychol. 53: 82–88.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Cropley, A. J. (1990).More Ways than One: Fostering Creativity, Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  9. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). The dangers of originality: Creativity and the artistic process. In Gedo, M. M. (ed.),Psychoanalytic Perspectives on Art, Analytic Press, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 213–224.Google Scholar
  10. Csikzentmihalyi, M. (1990). The domain of creativity. In Runco, M. A., and Albert, R. S. (eds.),Theories of Creativity, Sage, Newbury Park, pp. 190–212.Google Scholar
  11. Csikszentmihalyi, M., and Getzels, J. W. (1971). Discovery-oriented behavior and the originality of creative products: A study with artists.J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 19: 47–52.Google Scholar
  12. Dudek, S. Z., and Cote, R. (1994). Problem finding revisited. In Runco, M. A. (ed.),Problem Finding, Problem Solving, and Creativity, Ablex, Norwood, NJ, pp. 130–150.Google Scholar
  13. Dudek, S. Z., and Verrault, R. (1989). The creative thinking and ego functioning of children.Creat. Res. J. 2: 64–86.Google Scholar
  14. Elkind, D (1981).Children and Adolescents, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Eysenck, H. J. (1994). Creativity and personality: Word association, origence, and psychoticism.Creat. Res. J. 7: 209–216.Google Scholar
  16. Feldman, D. H., Marrinan, B. M., and Hartfeldt, S. D. (1972). Transformational power as a possible index of creativity.Psychol. Rep. 30: 335–338.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Finke, R. (in press). In Runco, M. A. (ed.),Creativity Research Handbook (Vol. 1), Hampton, Cresskill, NJ.Google Scholar
  18. Finke, R., Ward, T. B., and Smith, S. (1992).Creative Cognition: Theory, Research, and Applications, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  19. Firestien, R. L., and McCowan, R. J. (1989). Creative problems solving and communication behaviors in small groups.Creat. Res. J. 2: 106–114.Google Scholar
  20. Gedo, J. (1990). More on creativity and its vicissitudes. In Runco, M. A., and Albert, R. S.,Theories of creativity (pp. 35–45). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  21. Glicksohn, J., Kraemer, S, and Yisraeli, O. (1993). Metaphoric thinking and ideational fluency.Metaph. Symbol. Activ. 8.Google Scholar
  22. Guilford, J. P. (1967).The Nature of Human Intelligence, McGraw Hill, New York.Google Scholar
  23. Guilford, J. P. (1968).Creativity, Intelligence and Their Educational Implications, EDITS/Knapp, San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
  24. Guilford, J. P. (1983). Transformation abilities or functions.J. Creat. Behav. 17: 75–83.Google Scholar
  25. Harrington, D. M. (1975). Effects of explicit instructions to “be creative” on the psychological meaning of divergent thinking test scores.J. Person. 43: 434–454.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Harrington, D. M. (1981). Creativity, analogical thinking, and muscular metaphors.J. Ment. Imag. 6: 121–126.Google Scholar
  27. Hayes, J. R. (1981).Complete Problem Solver, Franklin, Philadelphia, PA.Google Scholar
  28. Heinzen, T. (1989). On moderate challenge increasing ideational creativity.Creat. Res. J. 2: 223–226.Google Scholar
  29. Hennessey, B. A. (1994). The consensual assessment technique: An examination of the relationship between ratings of product and process creativity.Creat. Res. J. 7: 193–208.Google Scholar
  30. Hocevar, D. (1979). Ideational fluency as a confounding factor in the measurement of originality.J. Educ. Psychol. 71: 191–196.Google Scholar
  31. Hocevar, D. (1981). Measurement of creativity: Review and critique.J. Person. Assess. 45: 450–464.Google Scholar
  32. Hogarth, R. (1987).Judgment and Choice (2nd Ed.), Wiley, Chichester.Google Scholar
  33. Jackson, P. W., and Messick, S. (1967). The person, the product, and the response: Conceptual problems in the assessment of creativity. In Kagan, J. (ed.),Creativity and Learning, Beacon Press, Boston, MA, pp. 1–15.Google Scholar
  34. Jausovec, N. (1989). Affect in analogical transfer.Creat. Res. J. 2: 255–266.Google Scholar
  35. Jausovec, N. (1994a).Flexibile Thinking: An Explanation for Individual Differences in Ability, Hamptom, Cresskil, NJ.Google Scholar
  36. Jausovec, N. (1994b). Metacognition in creative problem solving. In Runco, M. A. (ed.),Problem Finding, Problem Solving and Creativity, Ablex, Norwood, NJ, pp. 77–95.Google Scholar
  37. Jenkins, J. J. (1959). Effects of word association of the set to give popular responses.Psychol. Rep. 5: 94.Google Scholar
  38. Katz, A., and Thompson, M. (1993). On judging creativity: By one's acts shall ye be known.Creat. Res. J. 6: pp. 345–364.Google Scholar
  39. Kaufmann, G. (1979). The explorer and assimilator: A cognitive style distinction and its potential implications for innovative problem solving.Scand. J. Educ. Res. 23: 101–108.Google Scholar
  40. Kaufmann, G., and Raaheim, K. (1973). Effect of inducing activity upon performances in an unfamiliar task.Psychol. Rep. 32: 303–306.Google Scholar
  41. Khandwalla, P. N. (in press). An exploratory study of divergent thinking through protocol analysis.Creat. Res. J. Google Scholar
  42. Khatena, J., and Khatena, N. (1990). Metaphor motifs and creative imagination in art.Metaph. Symbol. Activ. 5: 21–34.Google Scholar
  43. Kogan, N. (1973). Creativity and cognitive style: A life-span perspective. In Baltes, P. P., and Schaie, K. W. (eds.),Life-Span Developmental Psychology: Personality and Socialization, New York, Academic Press, pp. 145–178.Google Scholar
  44. Kris, E. (1952).Psychoanalytic Explorations in Art. International Universities Press, New York.Google Scholar
  45. Langley, P., and Jones, R. (1988). A computational model of scientific insight. In Sternberg, R. (ed.),The Nature of Creativity: Contemporary Psychological Perspectives, Cambridge University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  46. Lasswell, H. D. (1959). The social setting of creativity. In Anderson, H. H.,Creativity and Its Cultivation, Harper, New York, pp. 203–221.Google Scholar
  47. Lazarus, R. S. (1991a). Cognition and motivation in emotion.Am. Psychol. 46: 352–367.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Lazarus, R. S. (1991b). Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion.Am. Psychol. 46: 819–834.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Mansfield, R. S., and Busse, T. V. (1981).The Psychology of Creativity and Discovery, Nelson-Hall, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
  50. Martindale, C., and Armstrong, J. (1974). The relationship of creativity to cortical activity.J. Genetic Psych, 124: 311–320.Google Scholar
  51. Martinsen, O. (1993). Insight problems revisited: The influence of cognitive styles and experience on creative problem solving.Creat. Res. J. 6: 435–447.Google Scholar
  52. Maslow, A. H. (1971).The Farther Reaches of Human Nature, Viking Press, New York.Google Scholar
  53. Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process.Psychol. Rev. 69: 22–232.Google Scholar
  54. Miall, D. S. (1987). Metaphor and affect: The problem of creative thought.Metaph. Symbol. Activ. 2: 81–96.Google Scholar
  55. Milgram, R. M., and Rabkin, L. (1980). Developmental test of Mednick's associative hierarchies of original thinking.Devel. Psychol. 16: 157–158.Google Scholar
  56. Mobley, M. I. Doares, M., and Mumford, M. D. (1993). Process analytic models of creative capacities: Evidence for the combination and reorganization process.Creat. Res. J. 3.Google Scholar
  57. Motley, M. (1980). Slips of the tongue.Sci. Am. 116–120, 125–126.Google Scholar
  58. Mumford, M. D. (1993). A new combination? Review of R. Finke et al.'sCreative cognition.Creat. Res. J. 6: 475–477.Google Scholar
  59. Mumford, M. D., and Gustafson, S. B. (1988). Creativity syndrome: Integration, application, and innovation.Psychol. Bull. 103: 27–43.Google Scholar
  60. Mumford, M. D., Costanza, D. P., Threlfall, K. V., Baughman, W. A., and Reiter-Palmon, R. (1993). Personality variables and problem construction activities: An exploratory investigation.Creat. Res. J. 6: 365–398.Google Scholar
  61. Nelson, D. (1982).City Building Education: A Way to Learn, Center for City Building Education Programs, Santa Monica, CA.Google Scholar
  62. Okuda, S. M., Runco, M. A., and Berger, D. E. (1991). Creativity and the finding and solving of real-world problems.J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 9: 45–53.Google Scholar
  63. Patrick, C. (1935). Creative thought in poets.Arch. Psychol. 26: 1–74.Google Scholar
  64. Patrick, C. (1937). Creative thought in artists.J. Psychol. 4: 35–73.Google Scholar
  65. Perkins, D. (1981).The Mind's Best Work, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  66. Proctor, R. A. (1989). The use of metaphors to aid in the process of creative problem solving.Person. Rev. 18.Google Scholar
  67. Rogers, C. (1970). Toward a theory of creativity. In Vernon, P. E. (ed.),Creativity, Penguin, New York, pp. 137–151.Google Scholar
  68. Rothberg, M. A. (1967). The effect of “social” instructions on word association behavior.J. Verb. Learn./Verb. Behav. 6: 298–300.Google Scholar
  69. Rothenberg, A. (1990).Creativity and Madness, Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD.Google Scholar
  70. Rothenberg, A., and Burkhardt, P. E. (1984). Differences in response time of creative persons and patients with depressive and schizophrenic disorders.Psychol. Rep. 54: 711–717.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Rothernberg, A., and Hausman, C. (1976).The Creativity Question, Duke University Press, Durham, NC.Google Scholar
  72. Rothenberg, A., and Hausman, C. (in press). Metaphor and creativity. In Runco, M. A. (ed.),Creativity Research Handbook (Vol. 2), Hampton, Cresskill, NJ.Google Scholar
  73. Rubenson, D. L., and Runco, M. A. (1992). The psychoeconomic approach to creativity.New Ideas Psychol. 10: 131–147.Google Scholar
  74. Runco, M. A. (1986). Maximal performance on divergent thinking tests by gifted, talented, and nongifted children.Psychol. Schools 23: 308–315.Google Scholar
  75. Runco, M. A. (1986a). Divergent thinking and creative performance in gifted and nongifted children.Educ. Psychol. Meas. 46: 375–384.Google Scholar
  76. Runco, M. A. (1986b). Flexibility and originality in children's divergent thinking.J. Psychol. 120: 345–352.Google Scholar
  77. Runco, M. A. (1986c). The discriminant validity of gifted children's divergent thinking test scores.Gifted Child Quart. 30: 78–82.Google Scholar
  78. Runco, M. A. (ed.) (1991a).Divergent Thinking, Ablex, Norwood, NJ.Google Scholar
  79. Runco, M. A. (1991b). The evaluative, valuative, and divergent thinking of children.J. Creat. Behav. 25: 311–319.Google Scholar
  80. Runco, M. A. (ed.) (1994a). Conclusions about problem finding, problem solving, and creativity. In Runco, M. A. (ed.),Problem Finding, Problem Solving, and Creativity, Ablex, Norwood, NJ, pp. 271–290.Google Scholar
  81. Runco, M. A. (1994b). Creativity and its discontents. In Shaw, M. P., and Runco, M. A. (eds.),Creativity and Affect, Ablex, Norwood, NJ, pp. 53–65.Google Scholar
  82. Runco, M. A. (in press a). Creativity, cognition, and their educational implications. In Houtz, J. C. (ed.),An Educational Psychology of Creativity, Fordham University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  83. Runco, M. A. (in press b). Objectivity in creativity research. In Montuori, M. (ed.),Unusual Associates: Essays in Honor of Frank Barron, Hampton, Cresskill, NJ.Google Scholar
  84. Runco, M. A., and Basadur, M. (1993). Assessing ideational and evaluative skills and creative styles and attitudes.Creat. Innov. Manage. 2: 166–173.Google Scholar
  85. Runco, M. A., and Chand, I. (1994). Problem finding, evaluative thinking, and creativity. In Runco, M. A. (ed.),Problem Finding, Problem Solving, and Creativity, Ablex, Norwood, NJ, pp. 40–76.Google Scholar
  86. Runco, M. A., and Charles, R. (1993). Judgments of originality and appropriateness as predictors of creativity.Person. Indiv. Diff. 15: 537–546.Google Scholar
  87. Runco, M. A., and Charles, R. (in press). Developmental trends in creativity. In Runco, M. A. (ed.),Creativity Research Handbook (Vol. 1), Hampton, Cresskill, NJ.Google Scholar
  88. Runco, M. A., and Okuda, S. M. (1988). Problem-discovery, divergent thinking, and the creative process.J. Youth Adolesc. 17: 211–220.Google Scholar
  89. Runco, M. A., and Okuda, S. M. (1991). The instructional enhancement of the ideational originality and flexibility scores of divergent thinking tests.App. Cognit. Psychol. 5: 435–441.Google Scholar
  90. Runco, M. A., and Smith, W. R. (1992). Interpersonal and intrapersonal evaluations of creative ideas.Person. Indiv. Diff. 13: 295–302.Google Scholar
  91. Runco, M. A., and Vega, L. (1990). Evaluating the creativity of children's ideas.J. Soc. Behav. Person.Google Scholar
  92. Runco, M. A., Ebersole, P., and Mraz, W. (1991a). Self-actualization and creativity.J. Soc. Behav. Person. 6: 161–167.Google Scholar
  93. Runco, M. A., Okuda, S. M., and Thurston, B. J. (1991b). Environmental cues and divergent thinking. In Runco, M. A. (ed.),Divergent Thinking, Ablex, Norwood, NJ, pp. 79–85.Google Scholar
  94. Runco, M. A., Johnson, D., and Gaynor, J. R. (in press). The judgmental bases for creativity and implications for gifted youth. In Fishkin, A. S., Crammond, B., and Olszewski-Kubilius, P. (eds.),Current Research and Methods for Studying Creativity in Youth, Hampton, Cresskill, NJ.Google Scholar
  95. Russ, S. (1992).Affect and Creativity, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
  96. Schuldberg, D. (1994). Giddiness and horror in the creative process. In Shaw, M. P., and Runco, M. A. (eds.),Creativity and Affect, Ablex, Norwood, NJ.Google Scholar
  97. Schwebel, M. (1993). Moral creativity as artistic transformation.Creat. Res. J. 6: 65–82.Google Scholar
  98. Simon, H. (1988). Creativity and motivation: A response to Csikszentmihalyi.New Ideas Psychol. 6: 177–181.Google Scholar
  99. Simon, H. A., and Chase, W. (1973). Skill in chess.Am. Sci. 61: 394–403.Google Scholar
  100. Simonton, D. K. (1984).Genius, Creativity, and Leadership, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  101. Simonton, D. K. (1988).Scientific Genius: A Psychology of Science, Cambridge University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  102. Smith, G. W., and van der Meer, G. (in press). Perception and creativity. In Runco, M. A. (ed.),Creativity Research Handbook (Vol. 1), Hampton, Cresskill, NJ.Google Scholar
  103. Toplyn, G., and Maguire, W. (1991). The differential effect of noise on creative task performance.Creat. Res. J. 4: 337–348.Google Scholar
  104. Torrance, E. P. (1968). A longitudinal examination of the fourth-grade slump in creativity.Gifted Child Quart. 12: 195–199.Google Scholar
  105. Wakefield, J. F. (1985). Towards creativity.Child Study J. 15: 265–270.Google Scholar
  106. Wechsler, S. (1985). Assessment of verbal creative strengths in Brazilian adults.School Psychol. Int. 6: 133–138.Google Scholar
  107. Weisberg, R. (1986).Creativity, Genius, and Other Myths, Freeman, New York.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1995

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mark A. Runco
    • 2
  • Ivonne Chand
    • 1
  1. 1.Claremont Graduate SchoolClaremontUSA
  2. 2.California State UniversityFullerton

Personalised recommendations