Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 20, Issue 1, pp 115–128

A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children

  • Peter Mundy
  • Marian Sigman
  • Connie Kasari
Article

Abstract

This study was designed to examine the degree to which individual differences in gestural joint attention skills predicted language development among autistic children. A group of 15 autistic children (mean CA=45 months) were matched with one group of mentally retarded (MR) children on mental age and another group of MR children on language age. These groups were administered the Early Social-Communication Scales. The latter provided measures of gestural requesting, joint attention, and social behaviors. The results indicated that, even when controlling for language level, mental age, or IQ, autistic children displayed deficits in gestural joint attention skills on two testing sessions that were 13 months apart. Furthermore, the measure of gestural nonverbal joint attention was a significant predictor of language development in the autistic sample. Other variables, including initial language level and IQ were not significant predictors of language development in this sample.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (1980).Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  2. Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in motherinfant and peer-infant interaction.Child Development, 55, 1278–1289.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bates, E. (1979).The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S. (1988). Social and pragmatic deficits in autism: Cognitive or affective?Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 379–401.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bruner, J., & Sherwood, V. (1983). Thought, language and interaction in infancy. In J. D. Call, E. Galenson, & R. L. Tyson (Eds.),Frontiers of infant psychiatry. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  6. Cattell, P. (1940).The measurement of intelligence of infants and young children. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  7. Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and communication in mute autistic children.Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 281–292.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Dawson, G., & Adams, A. (1984). Imitation and social responsiveness in autistic children.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 209–226.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Freeman, B., Ritvo, E., Needleman, R., & Yokota, A. (1985). The stability of cognitive and linguistic parameters in autism: A 5-year study.Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24, 290–311.Google Scholar
  10. Hannan, T. (1987). A cross-sequential assessment of the occurrences of pointing in 3- to 12-month-old human infants.Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 11–12.Google Scholar
  11. Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. (1990). Affective sharing in the context of attention interaction of normal, autistic, and mentally retarded children.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 87–100.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Landry, S., & Loveland, K. (1988). Communication behaviors in autism and developmental language delay.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 29, 621–634.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Leung, E., & Rheingold, H. (1981). Development of pointing as a social gesture.Developmental Psychology, 11, 215–220.Google Scholar
  14. Lotter, V. (1978). Follow up studies. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.),Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treatment (pp. 475–495). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  15. Loveland, K., & Landry, S. (1986). Joint attention in language in autism and developmental language delay.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 335–349.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. McDonald, M., Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1989). Psychometric scatter antistic preschoolers as measured by the Cattell.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 594–604.Google Scholar
  17. Mitchell, F. (1979). Interobserver agreement, reliability, and generalizability of data collected in observational studies.Psychological Bulletin, 86, 376–390.Google Scholar
  18. Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989a). Specifying the nature of the social impairment in autism. In G. Sawson (Ed.),Autism: New Perspectives on diagnosis, nature, and treatment (pp. 3–12). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  19. Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989b). The theoretical implications of joint attention deficits in autism.Development and Psychology, 1, 173–183.Google Scholar
  20. Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of nonverbal communication measures.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 657–669.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1987). Play and nonverbal communication correlates of language development in autistic children.Journl of Autism and Developmental Disabilites, 17, 349–363.Google Scholar
  22. Prizant, B., & Schuler, A. (1987). Facilitating communication: Theoretical foundations. In D. Cohen, A. Donnellan, & R. Paul (Eds.),Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 289–300). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  23. Reynell, J. (1977).Reynell developmental language scales. Windsor, England: NFER Publishing.Google Scholar
  24. Rheingold, H., Hay, D., & West, M. (1976). Sharing in the second year of life.Child Development, 83, 898–913.Google Scholar
  25. Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler, (Eds.),Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treatment. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  26. Rutter, M. (1985). The treatment of autistic children.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 193–214.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Seibert, J., Hogan, A., & Mundy, P. (1982). Assessing interactional competencies: The Early Social-Communication Scales.Infant Mental Health Journal, 3, 244–259.Google Scholar
  28. Sigman, M., Mundy, P., Sherman, T., Ungerer, J. (1986). Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children and their caregivers.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiarty, 27, 647–656.Google Scholar
  29. Sugarman, S. (1984). The development of preverbal communication. In R. L. Schiefelbusch & J. Pickar (Eds.),The acquisition of communicative competence. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  30. Thorndike, R. (1972).Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale: Norms tables. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  31. Tiegerman, E., & Primavera, L. (1984). Imitating the autistic child: Facilitating communicative gaze.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 27–38.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963).Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  33. Wetherby, A., & Pruning, C. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 364–377.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Peter Mundy
    • 1
  • Marian Sigman
    • 1
  • Connie Kasari
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychiatryUCLA Center for the Health SciencesLos Angeles

Personalised recommendations