Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 25, Issue 4, pp 415–433 | Cite as

Teaching theory of mind: A new approach to social skills training for individuals with autism

  • Sally Ozonoff
  • Judith N. Miller
Article

Abstract

This study examined the effectiveness of a social skills training program for normal-IQ adolescents with autism. Five boys participated in the 4 1/2-month treatment condition; four boys matched on age, IQ, and severity of autism constituted the no-treatment control group. In addition to teaching specific interactional and conversational skills, the training program provided expliciand systematic instruction in the underlying social-cognitive principles necessary to infer the mental states of others (i.e., theory of mind). Pre- and post-intervention assessment demonstrated meaningful change in the treatment group's performance on several false belief tasks, but no improvement in the control sample. No changes, however, were demonstrated on general parent and teacher ratings of social competence for either group.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Baer, L., & Ahern, D. K. (1993). Statistical problems with small sample size.American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 356.Google Scholar
  2. Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child's theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285–297.Google Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S., & Howlin, P. (1993). The theory of mind deficit in autism: Some questions for teaching and diagnosis. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.),Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 466–480). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind?≓Cognition, 21, 37–46.Google Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioral and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113–125.Google Scholar
  6. Berler, E. S., Gross, A. M., & Drabman, R. S. (1982). Social skills training with children: Proceed with caution.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, 41–53.Google Scholar
  7. Bowler, D. (1992). Theory of mind in Asperger's Syndrome.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 877–893.Google Scholar
  8. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer.Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.Google Scholar
  9. Goldstein, A. P. (1988).The Prepare Curriculum: Teaching prosocial competencies. Champaign, IL: Research Press.Google Scholar
  10. Gresham, F. M. (1983). Social validity in the assessment of children's social skills: Establishing standards for social competency.Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 1, 299–307.Google Scholar
  11. Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990).Social skills rating system. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  12. Happe, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154.Google Scholar
  13. Holroyd, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). Brief report: How far can people with autism go in developing a theory of mind?Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, 379–385.Google Scholar
  14. Hughes, J. N., & Sullivan, K. A. (1988). Outcome assessment in social skills training with children.Journal of School Psychology, 26, 167–183.Google Scholar
  15. Jacobson, N. S., Follette, W. C., & Revenstorf, D. (1984). Psychotherapy outcome research: Methods for reporting variability and evaluating clinical significance.Behavior Therapy, 15, 336–352.Google Scholar
  16. Kazdin, A. E. (1980).Research design in clinical psychology. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  17. La Greca, A. M. (1993). Social skills training with children: Where do we go from here?Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 288–298.Google Scholar
  18. Leekam, S., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a theory of representation?Cognition, 40, 203–218.Google Scholar
  19. Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1988). Autistic children's understanding of seeing, knowing and believing.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 315–324.Google Scholar
  20. Malik, N. M., & Furman, W. (1993). Problems in children's peer relations: What can the clinician do?Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1303–1326.Google Scholar
  21. Matson, J. L., & Swiezy, N. (1994). Social skills training with autistic children. In J. L. Matson (Ed.),Autism in children and adults: Etiology, assessment and intervention (pp. 241–260). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
  22. Mesibov, G. B. (1984). Social skills training with verbal autistic adolescents and adults: A program model.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 395–404.Google Scholar
  23. Mesibov, G. B. (1992). Treatment issues with high-functioning adolescents and adults with autism. In E. Schopler & G.B. Mesibov (Eds.),High-functioning individuals with autism (pp. 143–155). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  24. Ozer, D. J. (1985). Correlation and the coefficient of determination.Psychological Bulletin, 97, 307–315.Google Scholar
  25. Ozonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism.Development and Psychopathology, 6, 415–431.Google Scholar
  26. Paquin, M. J. R. (1983). Beyond significant but meaningless results in psychotherapy research.Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 20, 38–40.Google Scholar
  27. Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later personal adjustment: Are lowaccepted children at risk?Psychological Bulletin, 102, 357–389.Google Scholar
  28. Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. M., & Leekam, S. R. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: Knowledge, belief, and communication.Child Development, 60, 689–700.Google Scholar
  29. Rosnow, R. L., & Rosenthal, R. (1988). Focused tests of significance and effect size estimation in counseling psychology.Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35, 203–208.Google Scholar
  30. Rutter, M., & Bailey, A. (1993). Thinking and relationships: Mind and brain (some reflections on theory of mind and autism). In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D. Cohen (Eds.),Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 481–504). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Schopler, E., & Mesibov, G. B. (Eds.). (1983).Autism in adolescents and adults. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  32. Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1988).The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  33. Walker, H. M., McConnell, S., Holmes, D., Todis, B., Walker, B., & Golden, N. (1983).The Walker Social Skills Curriculum: The Accepts Program. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
  34. Williams, T. I. (1989). A social skills group for autistic children.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 143–155.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1995

Authors and Affiliations

  • Sally Ozonoff
    • 1
  • Judith N. Miller
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of UtahSalt Lake City

Personalised recommendations