Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 24, Issue 6, pp 719–735 | Cite as

Effects of high and low constraint utterances on the production of immediate and delayed echolalia in young children with autism

  • Patrick J. Rydell
  • Pat Mirenda
Article

Abstract

This study examined the effects of adult antecedent utterances on the occurrence and use of echolalia in children with autism during a free play setting. Adult antecedent utterances were differentiated into two types, high and low constraint, based on the degree of linguistic constraint inherent in the adult utterance and social-communicative control exerted on the child's social and verbal interaction. Results of this study identified a variety of patterns of echolalia usage following adult high and low constraint utterances. Overall results found that a majority of immediate echoes followed high constraint utterances and were primarily used as responsives, organizational devices, and cognitives. The majority of delayed echoes followed low constraint utterances and were primarily used as requestives, assertives, and cognitives. Delayed echoes were more likely than immediate echoes to be produced with evidence of comprehension, but there were no differences in comprehension within the two categories of echolalia following high and low constraint utterances. Educational implications are discussed.

Keywords

Young Child School Psychology Free Play Verbal Interaction Educational Implication 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Cantwell, D., Baker, L., & Rutter, M. (1978). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive languate disorder, IV: Analysis of syntax and function.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 351–363.Google Scholar
  2. Charlop, M. (1983). The effects of echolalia on acquisition and generalization of receptive labeling in autistic children.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 16, 111–126.Google Scholar
  3. Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1985).Test of Auditory Comprehension of Language (rev. ed.). Allen, TX:DLM Teaching Resources.Google Scholar
  4. Duchan, J. F. (1983). Autistic children are non-interactive: Or so we say.Seminars in Speech, Language and Hearing, 4, 53–61.Google Scholar
  5. Dore, J. (1986). The development of conversational competence. In R. Schiefelbusch (Ed.),Language competence: Assessment and intervention. San Diego: College-Hill.Google Scholar
  6. Fay, W., & Schuler, A. (1980).Emerging language in autistic children. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  7. Hermelin, B., & O'Connor, N. (1970).Psychological experiments with autistic children. London: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  8. Howlin, P. (1981). The effectiveness of operant language training with autistic children.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.11, 89–106.Google Scholar
  9. Hubbell, R. D. (1977). On facilitating spontaneous talking in young children.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 42, 216–231.Google Scholar
  10. McDonald, L., & Pien, D. (1982). Mother conversational behavior as a function of interactional intent.Journal of Child Language, 9, 337–358.Google Scholar
  11. McEvoy, R., Loveland, K., & Landry, S. (1988). The functions of immediate echolalia in autistic children: A developmental perspective.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 18, 657–668.Google Scholar
  12. Miller, J. F., & Chapman, R. S. (1981). The relation between age and mean length of utterance in morphemes.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 154–161.Google Scholar
  13. Miller, J. F. & Chapman, R. S. (1983).Systematic analysis of language transcripts (Apple version). Madison: University of Wisocosin-Madison. Mishler, E. G. (1979). Meaning in context: Is there any other kind?Harvard Educational Review, 49, 1–19.Google Scholar
  14. Prizant, B. M. (1983). Language and communication in autism: Toward an understanding of the “whole” of it.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 296–307.Google Scholar
  15. Prizant, B. M., & Duchan, J. F. (1981). The functions of immediate echolalia in autistic children.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46, 241–249.Google Scholar
  16. Prizant, B. M., & Rydell, P. J. (1984). An analysis of the functions of delayed echolalia in autistic children.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 183–192.Google Scholar
  17. Prizant, B. M. & Rydell, P. J. (1993). Assessment and intervention considerations for unconventional verbal behavior. In J. Reichle & D. Wacker (Eds.),Communicative alternatives to challenging behavior (pp. 263–297). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
  18. Rimland, B. (1964).Diagnostic Check List for Behavior-Disturbed Children: Form E-2. San Diego, CA:Google Scholar
  19. Roberts, J. (1989). Echolalia and Comprehension in Autistic children.journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 271–281.Google Scholar
  20. Rydell, P. J. (1989).Social-communicative control and its effect on echolalia in children with autism. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Nebraska-Lincoln.Google Scholar
  21. Rydell, P. J., & Mirenda, P. (1991). The effects of two levels of linguistic constraint on echolalia and generative language production in children with autism.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 1991.Google Scholar
  22. Schreibman, L., & Carr, E. (1978). Elimination of echolalic responding to questions through the training of a generalized verbal response.Journal of Applied Behavior Analyss, 11, 453–464.Google Scholar
  23. Schuler, A. L. (1979). Echolalia: Issues and clinical applications.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 44, 411–434.Google Scholar
  24. Schuler, A. L., & Prizant, B. M. (1985). Echolalia. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.),Communication problems in autism. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  25. Seibert, J., & Oller, D. (1981). Linguistic pragmatics and language intervention strategies.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 75–88.Google Scholar
  26. Shatz, B. (1977). The relationship between cognitive processes and the development of communication skills. In B. Keasey (Ed.),Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 1–42). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  27. Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1984).Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Interview ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1994

Authors and Affiliations

  • Patrick J. Rydell
    • 1
  • Pat Mirenda
    • 2
  1. 1.Autism Communication ServicesLittleton
  2. 2.CBI Consultants, Ld.Vancouver

Personalised recommendations