Journal of Youth and Adolescence

, Volume 8, Issue 2, pp 131–147

A longitudinal study of adolescents' future orientation (time perspective)

  • Gisela Trommsdorff
  • Helmut Lamm
  • Rolf W. Schmidt
Article

Abstract

This longitudinal study was designed to test hypotheses concerning the influence of environmental changes in different groups (educational status; sex) of adolescents on their future orientation (FO). FO was measured through a free-response questionnaire on which the subjects indicated their hopes and fears (which were later categorized into various domains of life by independent judges). On a second questionnaire the subjects adjudged their present and future life in various given domains of life. The sample consisted of 24 male and 24 female adolescents, aged between 14–16 years at the time of the first measurement; the second measurement took place two years later. Half of the subjects of each sex were students at a Gymnasium (preparing for university study) at both times of measurement (“high social status”). The other half of the subjects of each sex were in their last year at the Hauptschule (continuation of elementary education) at the first time of measurement, and had taken up employment (e.g., an apprenticeship) by the time of the second measurement (“low social status”). At the time of the second measurement relatively more hopes and fears were voiced in the domains of personal development and occupation than at the first measurement. Independent of time of measurement, low-status subjects voiced relatively more hopes and fears related to the occupational domain, and boys had a more extended FO than girls. Low-status subjects — as compared to high-status subjects as well as to their own judgments at the first measurement — believed to a greater extent that the materialization of their hopes and fears depended on themselves rather than on external forces. These results support the assumption that FO cannot be dealt with as a global concept and that FO can be modified by change in the external environments, such as entry into an occupation.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Atchley, R. C. (1975). The life course, age grading, and age-linked demands for decision making. In Datan, N., and Ginsberg, L. H. (eds.),Life Span Developmental Psychology: Normative Life Crises Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  2. Barabasz, A. F. (1970). Temporal orientation and academic achievement in college.J. Soc. Psychol. 80: 231–232.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bergius, R. (1957).Formen des Zukunftserlebens Barth, Munich.Google Scholar
  4. Brannigan, G. G., and Tolor, A. (1971). Sex differences in adaptive styles.J. Genet. Psychol. 119: 143–149.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Cantril, H. (1965).The Pattern of Human Concerns Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick, N. J.Google Scholar
  6. Doob, L. W. (1971).Patterning of Time Yale University Press, New Haven.Google Scholar
  7. Fraisse, P. (1957).Psychologie du temps. Presses Universitaires, Paris.Google Scholar
  8. Heckhausen, H. (1967).The Anatomy of Achievement Motivation Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  9. Jessor, R., Graves, T. D., Hanson, R. C., and Jessor, S. L. (1968).Society, Personality and Deviant Behavior Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York.Google Scholar
  10. Kastenbaum, R. (1961). The dimensions of future time perspective: An experimental analysis.J. Gen. Psychol. 65: 203–218.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Klineberg, S. L. (1967). Changes in outlook on the future between childhood and adolescence.J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 7: 185–193.Google Scholar
  12. Lamm, H., Schmidt, R. W., and Trommsdorff, G. (1976). Sex and social class as determinants of future orientation (time perspective) in adolescents.J. Personal Soc. Psychol. 34: 317–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Lehr, U. (1972). Das Problem geschlechtsspezifischer Verhaltensweisen. In Graumann, C. F. (ed.),Handbuch der Psychologie, Vol. 7, No. 2, Hogrefe, Göttingen.Google Scholar
  14. LeShan, L. L. (1952). Time orientation and social class.J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 47: 589–592.Google Scholar
  15. Lessing, E. E. (1971). Comparative extension of personal and social political future time perspective.Percept. Motor Skills 33: 415–422.Google Scholar
  16. Lessing, E. E. (1972). Extension of personal future time perspective, age and life satisfaction of children and adolescents.Dev. Psychol. 6: 457–468.Google Scholar
  17. Lewin, K. (1948). Time perspective and morale. In Lewin, K. (ed.),Resolving Social Conflicts Harper, New York, pp. 103–124.Google Scholar
  18. Lewin, K. (1951).Field Theory in Social Science. Harper, New York.Google Scholar
  19. Mönks, F. J. (1967). Sammelreferat: Zeitperspektive als psychologische Variable.Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie 119: 131–161.Google Scholar
  20. Nuttin, J. (1964).The Future Time Perspective in Human Motivation and Learning: Proceedings of the 17th International Congress of Psychology North-Holland Publishing Co., Amsterdam.Google Scholar
  21. O'Rand, A., and Ellis, R. A. (1974). Social class and social time perspective.Soc. Forces 53: 53–62.Google Scholar
  22. Raynor, J. O. (1974). Future orientation in the study of achievement motivation. In Atkinson, J. W. and Raynor, J. O. (eds.),Motivation and Achievement Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  23. Roberts, A. H., and Greene, J. E. (1971). Cross-cultural study of relationship among four dimensions of time perspective.Percept. Motor Skills 33: 163–173.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement.Psychol. Monogr. 80(1): Whole No. 609.Google Scholar
  25. Schmidt, R. W. (1973).Soziale Determinanten der Zukunftsperspektive. Eine Untersuchung an berufstätigen Männern und Frauen im Alter zwischen 35 und 45 Jahren. Unveröffentlichte Diplomarbeit.Google Scholar
  26. Teahan, J. E. (1958). Future time perspective, optimism, and academic achievement.J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 57: 379–380.Google Scholar
  27. Toban, E. V. (1970). Relation between future time perspective, age and militancy.J. Genet. Psychol. 116: 63–68.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Trommsdorff, G. (1978). Social conditions of future orientation. Paper presented to the European Association of Social Psychology, Weimar.Google Scholar
  29. Trommsdorff, G., and Lamm, H. (1975). An analysis of future orientation and some of its social determinants. In Fraser, J. T., and Lawrence, N. (eds.),The Study of Time, II. Springer, Heidelberg/New York, Pp. 343–361.Google Scholar
  30. Trommsdorff, G., Burger, Ch., Füchsle, T., and Lamm, H. (1978).Erziehung für die Zukunft. Schwann, Düsseldorf.Google Scholar
  31. Wallace, M. (1956). Future time perspective in schizophrenia.J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 52: 240–245.Google Scholar
  32. Weiner, B. (1974).Achievement Motivation and Attribution Theory General Learning Press, Morristown, N. J.Google Scholar
  33. Winer, B. J. (1971).Statistical Principles in Experimental Design McGraw-Hill, New York.Google Scholar
  34. Winnubst, J. A. M. (1975).Het westerse tijdssyndroom. Conceptuele integratie en eerste aanzet tot construct validatie van een reeks molaire tijdsvariabelen in die psychologie. Swetz & Zeitlinger, Amsterdam.Google Scholar
  35. Zurcher, L. A., Jr., Willis, J. E., Ikard, F. F., and Dohme, J. A. (1967). Dogmatism, future orientation and perception of time.J. Soc. Psychol. 73: 205–209.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1979

Authors and Affiliations

  • Gisela Trommsdorff
    • 3
  • Helmut Lamm
    • 1
  • Rolf W. Schmidt
    • 2
  1. 1.Pädagogische HochschuleCologne
  2. 2.Speyer
  3. 3.Universität MannheimMannheimWest Germany

Personalised recommendations