Advertisement

Experientia

, Volume 41, Issue 1, pp 122–125 | Cite as

Short-distance homing in a small mammal: the role of exteroceptive cues and path integration

  • A. S. Etienne
  • E. Teroni
  • R. Maurer
  • V. Portenier
  • F. Saucy
Short Communications Neurology, Behavior

Summary

When hoarding food under IR light, the golden hamster returns to its nest by path integration after an active outward journey, and it is capable of compensating the angular component of a passive outward journey independently of auditory, olfactory, tactile and geomagnetic cues. If, however, peripheral visual cues are available, they predominate over information which is gained during the active or passive outward journey. Further experiments show the limitations of homing by path integration, which is open to cumulative errors and therefore needs to be complemented by other categories of information.

Key words

Hamster, golden Mesocricetus auratus homing, short distance exteroceptive cues path integration 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Literatur

  1. 2.
    Watson, J. B., Psychol. Rev. Monogr. suppl.8 (2) (1907).Google Scholar
  2. 3.
    Görner, R., Fortschr Zool.21 (1973) 20.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. 4.
    Mittelstaedt, H., and Mittelstaedt, M.-L., Fortschr. Zool.21 (1973) 46.Google Scholar
  4. 5.
    Mittelstaedt, H., Fortschr. Zool.28 (1983) 197.Google Scholar
  5. 6.
    Etienne, A. S., Experientia36 (1980) 1048.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. 7.
    Beritachvili (Beritov), I. S., Neuropsychologia1 (1963) 233Google Scholar
  7. 8.
    Potegal, M., in: Spatial Abilities, Development and Physiological Foundations, p. 361. Ed. M. Potegal. Academic Press, New York and London 1982.Google Scholar
  8. 9.
    Barlow, J. S., J. theor. Biol.6 (1964) 76.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. 10.
    Mayne, R., in: Handbook of Sensory Physiology, vol. 6 (2), p. 493. Ed. H. H. Kornhuber. Springer, Berlin 1974.Google Scholar
  10. 11.
    Vauclair, M., Gramoni, H., Meyer, J.-J., and Zinder, M., Biol. Behav.2 (1977) 353.Google Scholar
  11. 12.
    Batschelet, E., Statistical methods for the analysis of problems in animal orientation and certain biological rhythms. The American Institute of Biological Sciences, Washington D. C. 1965.Google Scholar
  12. 13.
    Mather, J. G., and Barker, R. R., Nature 291 (1981) 152.Google Scholar
  13. 14.
    Wiltschko, W., and Wiltschko, R., Science176 (1972) 62.Google Scholar
  14. 15.
    Bischof, N., in: Handbuch der Psychologie, vol. 1, p. 409. Ed. W. Metzger. Hofgrefe, Göttingen 1966.Google Scholar
  15. 16.
    Mittelstaedt, H., and Mittelstaedt, M.-L., in: Avian Navigation, p. 290. Eds F. Papi and H. G. Wallraff. Springer, Berlin 1982.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Birkhäuser Verlag 1985

Authors and Affiliations

  • A. S. Etienne
    • 1
  • E. Teroni
    • 1
  • R. Maurer
    • 1
  • V. Portenier
    • 1
  • F. Saucy
    • 1
  1. 1.Laboratoire d'Ethologie, F.P.S.E.University of GenevaSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations