Advertisement

Neuropsychology Review

, Volume 6, Issue 4, pp 203–257 | Cite as

Can we learn from the clinically significant face processing deficits, prosopagnosia and capgras delusion?

  • Elaine Wacholtz
Article

Abstract

This review describes two clinically significant face processing deficits, prosopagnosia and Capgras delusion, and provides new knowledge about the face recognition process by a convergence of empirical findings. These empirical findings are structured around two questions that are viewed from the perspectives of the two deficits. First is the question of hemispheric specificity, which inquires into the degree of each hemisphere's contribution to the face recognition process. Second is the question of dual neural pathways, which addresses the possibility that the face recognition process proceeds along two parallel pathways in the brain. Findings from the hemispheric specificity studies reinforce the current view that right hemisphere involvement is necessary for face recognition while left hemisphere involvement is minimal. Findings from the dual neural pathways studies reinforce the plausible but yet unproven hypothesis that two neural pathways pass information from the visual association cortex in the occipital lobe toward the temporal lobes and limbic system when faces are seen and recognized. These findings, which also indicate that each of the dual neural pathways carries different, nonredundant information, could be instrumental in showing that the pathways play different roles in the manifestations of the clinically significant face processing deficits, prosopagnosia and Capgras delusion.

Key words

prosopagnosia Capgras delusion face recognition dual neural pathway hemispheric specificity 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aggleton, J. P. (1992). The functional effects of amygdala lesions in humans: A comparison with findings from monkeys. In Aggleton, J. P. (ed.),The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction, Wiley-Liss, Inc., pp. 458–503.Google Scholar
  2. Aggleton, J. P. (1993). The contribution of the amygdala to normal and abnormal emotional states.Trends in Neurosciences 16: 328–333.Google Scholar
  3. Alexander, M. P., Stuss, D. T., and Benson, D. F. (1979). Capgras syndrome: A reduplicative phenomenon.Neurology 29: 334–339.Google Scholar
  4. Anderson, D. N. (1988). The delusion of inanimate doubles: Implications for understanding the Capgras phenomenon.The British Journal of Psychiatry 153: 604–699.Google Scholar
  5. Anderson, D. N. (1990). Understanding Capgras syndrome [letter].The British Journal of Psychiatry 157: 622–623.Google Scholar
  6. Assal, G. Favue, D., and Anderes, J. P. (1984). Non-reconnaissance d'animaux familiers chez un paysan.Revue Neurologique 140: 580–584.Google Scholar
  7. Bauer, R. M. (1982). Visual hypoemotionality as a symptom of visual-limbic disconnection in man.Archives of Neurology 39: 702–708.Google Scholar
  8. Bauer, R. M. (1984). Autonomic recognition of names and faces: A neuropsychological application of the Guilty Knowledge Test.Neuropsychologia 22: 457–469.Google Scholar
  9. Bauer, R. M. (1986). The cognitive psychophysiology of prosopagnosia. In Ellis, H. D., Jeeves, M. A., Newcombe, F., and Young, A. (eds.),Aspects of Face Processing, Dordrecht, Holland, Martinus Nijhoff, pp. 253–267.Google Scholar
  10. Bauer, R. M., and Trobe, J. D. (1984). Visual memory and perceptual impairments in prosopagnosia.The Journal of Clinical Neuro-ophthalmology 4: 39–46.Google Scholar
  11. Bauer, R. M., and Verfaellie, M. (1988). Electrodermal discrimination of familiar but not unfamiliar faces in prosopagnosia.Brain and Cognition 8: 240–252.Google Scholar
  12. Bear, D. M. (1983) Hemispheric specialization and the neurology of emotion.Archives of Neurology 40: 195–203.Google Scholar
  13. Benson, D. F., and Stuss, D. T. (1990). Frontal lobe influences on delusions: A clinical perspective.Schizophrenia Bulletin 16: 403–411.Google Scholar
  14. Benson, D. F., Segarra, J., and Ray, W. J. (1974). Visual agnosia-prosopagnosia: A clinico-pathological correlation.Archives of Neurology 30: 307–310.Google Scholar
  15. Benton, A. L. (1980). The neuropsychology of face recognition.American Psychologist 35: 176–186.Google Scholar
  16. Benton, A. L. (1985). Facial recognition in 1984. In Costa, L., and Spreen, O. (eds.),Studies in Neuropsychology: Selected Papers of Arthur Benton, New York, Oxford University Press pp. 224–235.Google Scholar
  17. Benton, A. L. (1990). Facial recognition 1990.Cortex 26: 491–499.Google Scholar
  18. Benton, A. L., and Van Allen, M. W. (1972). Prosopagnosia and facial discrimination.Journal of the Neurological Sciences 15: 167–172.Google Scholar
  19. Berson, R. J. (1983). Capgras' syndrome.American Journal of Psychiatry 140: 969–978.Google Scholar
  20. Bidault, J. P., Luauté, A., and Tzavaras, A. (1986). Prosopagnosia and the delusional misidentification syndromes.Bibliotheca Psychiatrica 164: 80–91.Google Scholar
  21. Bodamer, J. (1947). Die Prosop-Agnosie.Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 179: 6–54.Google Scholar
  22. Bornstein, B. (1963). Prosopagnosia. In Halpern, L. (ed.).Problems of Dynamic Neurology, Jerusalem, Hadassah Medical Organization, pp. 283–318.Google Scholar
  23. Bornstein, B., Sroka, H., and Munitz, H. (1969). Prosopagnosia with animal face agnosia.Cortex 5: 164–169.Google Scholar
  24. Bredart, S., and Bruyer, R. (1994). The cognitive approach to familiar face processing in human subjects.Behavioral Processes.Google Scholar
  25. Bruce, V., and Young, A. (1986). Understanding face recognition.British Journal of Psychology 77: 305–327.Google Scholar
  26. Bruyer, R. (1991). Covert face recognition in prosopagnosia: A review.Brain and Cognition 15: 223–235.Google Scholar
  27. Bruyer, R. (1993). Failures of face processing in normal and brain-damaged subjects. In Boller, F., and Grafman, J. (eds.),Handbook of Neuropsychology (Vol. VII), Amsterdam, Elsevier, pp. 409–433.Google Scholar
  28. Bruyer, R. 1994. Face recognition. In Ramachandran, V. S. (ed.)Encyclopedia of Human Behavior (Vol 2), Academic Press, Inc., San Diego, CA, pp. 351–359.Google Scholar
  29. Bruyer, R., and Schweich, M. (1991). A clinical test battery of face processing.International Journal of Neuroscience 61: 19–30.Google Scholar
  30. Bruyer, R., Latterre, C., Seron, X., Feyereisen P., Strypstein, E., Pierrard, E., and Rectem, D. (1983). A case of prosopagnosia with some preserved covert remembrane of familiar faces.Brain and Cognition 2: 257–284.Google Scholar
  31. Burns, A. (1985). The oldest patient with Capgras syndrome.The British Journal of Psychiatry 147: 719–720.Google Scholar
  32. Burns, A. (1987). Capgras' syndrome in a patient with dementia [letter].The British Journal of Psychiatry 150: 876–877.Google Scholar
  33. Burton, A. M., and Bruce, V. (1992). I recognize your face but I can't remember your name: A simple explanation.British Journal of Psychology 83: 45–60.Google Scholar
  34. Burton, A. M., Bruce, V., and Johnston, R. A. (1990). Understanding face recognition with an interactive activation model.The British Journal of Psychology 81: 361–380.Google Scholar
  35. Burton, A. M., Young, A. W., Bruce, V., Johnston, R., and Ellis, A. W. (1991). Understanding covert recognition.Cognition 39: 129–166.Google Scholar
  36. Capgras, J., and Reboul-Lachaux, J. (1923). L'illusion des “sosies” dans un delire systematise chronique [The illusion of doubles as a chronicially systematized delusion].Bulletin de la Societé Clinique de Medicine Mentale 11: 6–16.Google Scholar
  37. Christodoulou, G. N. (1977). The syndrome of Capgras.British Journal of Psychiatry 130: 556–564.Google Scholar
  38. Christodoulou, G. N. (1991). The delusional misidentification syndromes.British Journal of Psychiatry 159: 65–69.Google Scholar
  39. Cohen, G., and Faulkner, D. (1986). Memory for names: Age differences in retrieval.British Journal of Developmental Psychology 4: 187–197.Google Scholar
  40. Collins, M. H., Hawthorne, M. E., Gribbin, N., and Jacobson, R. (1990). Capgras' syndrome with organic disorders.Postgraduate Medical Journal 66: 1064–1067.Google Scholar
  41. Crawford, J., Besson, J., Ellis, H., Parker, D., Salzen, E., Gemmel, H., Sharp, P., Bovine, D., and Smith, F. (1986). Facial processing in the dementias. In Ellis, H. D., Newcombe, F., and Young, A. W. (eds.),Aspects of Face Processing, Nijhoff, Dordrecht, pp. 293–301.Google Scholar
  42. Crichton, P., and Lewis, S. (1990) Delusional misidentification, AIDS, and the right hemisphere.British Journal of Psychiatry 157: 608–610.Google Scholar
  43. Critchley, E. M. R. (1989). The neurology of familiarity.Behavioral Neurology 2: 195–200.Google Scholar
  44. Cutting, J. (1990).The Right Cerebral Hemisphere and Psychiatric Disorders, Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  45. Cutting, J. (1991). Delusional misidentification and the role of the right hemisphere in the appreciation of identity.British Journal of Psychiatry 159: 70–75.Google Scholar
  46. Damasio, A. R. (1985). Prosopagnosia.Trends in Neurosciences, 8, 132–135Google Scholar
  47. Damasio, A. R. (1989). Neural mechanisms. In Ellis, H. D., and Young, A. W. (eds.),Handbook of Research on Face Processing, North Holland, Amsterdam, pp. 268–272.Google Scholar
  48. Damasio, A. R., and Damasio, H. (1986). The anatomical substrate of prosopagnosia. In Bruyer, R. (ed.),The Neuropsychology of Face Perception and Facial Expression, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 31–38.Google Scholar
  49. Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., and Tranel, D. (1986). Prosopagnosia: Anatomic and physiologic aspects. In Ellis, H. D., Jeeves, M. A., Newcombe, F., and Young, A. (eds.),Aspects of Face Processing, Martinus Nijhoff, Dordrecht, Holland, pp. 243–252.Google Scholar
  50. Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., and Van Hoesen, G. W. (1982). Prosopagnosia: Anatomic basis and behavior mechanisms.Neurology 32: 331–341.Google Scholar
  51. Damasio, A. R., Lima, A., and Damasio, H. (1975). Nervous function after right hemisphere hemispherectomy.Neurology 25: 89–93.Google Scholar
  52. Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., and Damasio, H. (1990). Face agnosia and the neural substrates of memory. In Maxwell Cowan, W. (ed.),Annual Review of Neuroscience, Vol. 13, pp. 89–109.Google Scholar
  53. De Hann, E. H. F., Bauer, R. M., and Greve, K. W. (1992). Behavioral and physiological evidence for covert face recognition in a prosopagnosic patient.Cortex 28: 77–95.Google Scholar
  54. De Haan, E. H. F., Young, A., and Newcombe, F. (1987a). Faces interfere with name classification in a prosopagnosic patient.Cortex 23: 309–316.Google Scholar
  55. Damasio, A. R., Eslinger, P. J., Damasio, H., Van Hoesen, G. W., and Cornell, S. (1985). Multimodal amnesic syndrome following bilateral temporal and basal forebrain damage: the case of patient DRB.Archives of Neurolology 42: 252–259.Google Scholar
  56. De Haan, E. H. F., Young, A., and Newcombe, F. (1987b). Face recognition without awareness.Cognitive Neuropsychology 4: 385–415.Google Scholar
  57. Della Sala, S., Muggia, S., Spinnler, H., and Zuffi, M. (1995). Cognitive modelling of face processing: Evidence from Alzheimer patients.Neuropsychologia 33: 675–687.Google Scholar
  58. De Renzi, E. (1986a). Current issues on prosopagnosia. In Ellis, H. D. Jeeves, M. A., Newcombe, F., and Young, A. (eds.),Aspects of Face Processing, Martinus Nijhoff, Dordrecht, Holland, pp. 243–252.Google Scholar
  59. De Renzi, E. (1986b). Prosopagnosia in two patients with CT scan evidence of damage confined to the right hemisphere.Neuropsychologia 24: 385–389.Google Scholar
  60. De Renzi, E. (1989). Prosopagnosia: A multi-stage, specific disorder? In Young, A. W., and Ellis, H. D. (eds.),The Handbook of Research on Face Processing, Amsterdam: North Holland, pp. 27–35.Google Scholar
  61. De Renzi, E., Faglioni, P., Grossi, D., and Nichelli, P. (1991). Apperceptive and associative forms of prosopagnosia.Cortex 27: 213–221.Google Scholar
  62. De Renzi, E., Perani, D., Carlesimo, G. A., Silveri, M. C., and Fazio, F. (1994). Prosopagnosia can be associated with damage confined to the right hemisphere—An MRI and PET study and a review of literature.Neuropsychologia 32: 893–902.Google Scholar
  63. Derombies, M. (1935). L'illusion de sosie, forme particulière de al méconnaisance systématique. InThèse de Paris. Jouve and Co., Paris.Google Scholar
  64. Dixon, N. F. (1984). Subliminal perception and microgenesis. In Froelich, W. D., Smith, G., Draguns, J., et al. (eds.),Psychological Processes in Cognition and Personality, Hemisphere Publishing Corporation, Washington, pp. 225–230.Google Scholar
  65. Doran, J. M. (1990). The Capgras syndrome: Neurological/neuropsychological perspectives.Neuropsychology 4: 29–42.Google Scholar
  66. Ellis, H. D. (1983). The role of the right hemisphere in face perception. In Young, A. W. (ed),Functions of the Right Hemisphere, Academic Press, London, pp. 33–64.Google Scholar
  67. Ellis, H. D. (1986). Introduction: Processes underlying face recognition. In Bruyer, R. (ed),The Neuropsychology of Face Perception and Facial Expression, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp. 1–27.Google Scholar
  68. Ellis, H. D. (1989). Past and recent studies of prosopagnosia. In Crawford, J. R., and Parker, D. M. (eds.),Developments in Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 151–166.Google Scholar
  69. Ellis, H. D. (1994). The role of the right hemisphere in the Capgras delusion.Psychopathology 27: 177–185.Google Scholar
  70. Ellis, H. D., and De Pauw, K. W. (1994). The cognitive neuropsychiatric origins of Capgras delusion. In David, A., and Cutting, J. (eds.),Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia, Erlbaum, Hove.Google Scholar
  71. Ellis, H. D., and Shepherd, J. W. (1992) Face memory—memory and practice. In Gruneberg, M., and Morris, P. (eds.),Aspects of Memory, Routledge, London, pp. 51–85.Google Scholar
  72. Ellis, H. D., De Pauw, K. W., Christodoulou, G. N., Papageorgiou, L., Milne, A. B., and Joseph, A. B. (1993). Responses to facial and non-facial stimuli presented tachistoscopically in either or both visual fields by patients with Capgras delusion and paranoid schizophrenics.Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 56: 215–219.Google Scholar
  73. Ellis H. D., and Young, A. W. (1990). Accounting for delusional misidentifications.British Journal of Psychiatry 157: 239–248.Google Scholar
  74. Ellis, H. D., and Young, A. W. (1994). Problems of person perception in schizophrenia. In Pantelis, C., Nelson, H. E., and Barnes, T. R. E. (eds.),Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia, John Wiley & Sons, Chichester.Google Scholar
  75. Ellis, H. D., Whitley, J., and Luauté, J-P. (1994). Delusional misidentification: The three original papers on the Capgras, Frégoli and intermetamorphosis delusions.History of Psychiatry 117–146.Google Scholar
  76. Ellis, A. W., Young, A. W., and Critchley, E. M. R. (1989). Loss of memory for people following temporal lobe damage.Brain 112: 1469–1483.Google Scholar
  77. Enoch, D. M., and Trethowan, S. W. (1991). Capgras syndrome. InUncommon Psychiatric Syndromes (3rd edn.), Butterworth-Heinemann, Oxford. pp. 1–23.Google Scholar
  78. Etcoff, N. L., Freeman, R., and Cave, K. R. (1991). Can we lose memories of faces? Content specificity and awareness in a prosopagnosic.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 3: 25–47.Google Scholar
  79. Ettlin, T. M., Beckson, M., Benson, D. F., Langfitt, J. T., Amos, E. C., and Pineda, G. S. (1992). Prosopagnosia: A bihemispheric disorder.Cortex 28: 129–134.Google Scholar
  80. Farah, M. J. (1990).Visual Agnosia: Disorders of Object Recognition and What They Tell Us about Normal Vision, The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  81. Feinberg, T. E., and Shapiro, R. M. (1989). Misidentification-reduplication and the right hemisphere.Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology 2: 39–48.Google Scholar
  82. Fishbain, D. A. (1987). The frequency of Capgras delusions in a psychiatric emergency service.Psychopathology 20: 42–47.Google Scholar
  83. Fishbain, D. A. (1989). The Capgras syndrome and minor head trauma.Journal of Clinical Psychology 50: 474–475.Google Scholar
  84. Fleminger, S. (1992). Seeing is believing: The role of ‘preconscious’ perceptual processing in delusional misidentification.British Journal of Psychiatry 160: 292–303.Google Scholar
  85. Flude, B. M., Ellis, A. W., and Kay, J. (1989). Face processing and name retrieval in an anomic aphasic: Names are stored separately from semantic information about familiar people.Brain and Cognition 11: 60–72.Google Scholar
  86. Förstl, H. (1990). Capgras' delusion: An example of coalescent psychodynamic and organic factors.Comprehensive Psychiatry 31: 447–449.Google Scholar
  87. Förstl, H., Almeida, O., and Iacoponi, E. (1991). Capgras delusion in the elderly: The evidence for a possible organic origin.International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 6: 845–852.Google Scholar
  88. Förstl, H., Burns, A., Jacoby, R., and Levy, R. (1991). Neuroanatomical correlates of clinical misidentification and misperception in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type.Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 52: 268–271.Google Scholar
  89. Gloning, I., Gloning, K., Hoff, H., et al. (1966). Zur Prosopagnosie.Neurophsychologia 4: 113–132.Google Scholar
  90. Gross, C. G., and Sergent, J. (1992). Face recognition.Current Biology 2: 156–161.Google Scholar
  91. Gross, C. G., Bender, D. B., and Gerstein, G. L. (1979). Activity of inferior temporal neurons in behaving monkeys.Neuropsychologia 17: 315–229.Google Scholar
  92. Hay, G. G. (1986). Electroconvulsive therapy as a contributor to the production of delusional misidentification.British Journal of Psychiatry 148: 667–669.Google Scholar
  93. Hécaen, H. (1981). The neuropsychology of face recognition. In Davies, G., Ellis, H., and Shepherd, J. (eds.),Perceiving and Remembering Faces, Academic Press, London, pp. 39–54.Google Scholar
  94. Hécaen, H., and Angelergues, R. (1962). Agnosia for faces (prosopagnosia).Archives of Neurology, Chicago 7: 92–100.Google Scholar
  95. Joseph, A. B. (1986a). Capgras syndrome [letter].The British Journal of Psychiatry 148: 749–750.Google Scholar
  96. Joseph, A. B. (1986b). Focal central nervous system abnormalities in patients with misidentification syndromes.Bibliotheca Psychiatrica 164: 68–79.Google Scholar
  97. Joseph, A. B., O'Leary, D. H., and Wheeler, H. G. (1990). Bilateral atrophy of the frontal and temporal lobes in schizophrenic patients with Capgras syndrome: A case-control study using computed tomography.Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 51: 322–325.Google Scholar
  98. Kapur, N., Turner, A., and King, C. (1988). Reduplicative paramnesia: Possible anatomical and neuropsychological mechanisms.Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 51: 579–581.Google Scholar
  99. Koritar, E., and Steiner, W. (1988). Capgras' syndrome: A synthesis of various viewpoints.Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 33: 62–66.Google Scholar
  100. Kosslyn, S. M. (1994).Image and Brain. The MIT Press. Cambridge, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  101. Kosslyn, S. M., and Koenig, O. (1992).Wet Mind. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
  102. Kumar, V. (1987). Capgras syndrome in a patient with dementia.The British Journal of Psychiatry 150: 251.Google Scholar
  103. Kurucz, J., and Feldmar, G. (1979). Prosopo-affective agnosia as a symptom of cerebral organic disease.Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 27: 225–230.Google Scholar
  104. Landis, T., Cummings, J. L., Christen, L., Bogen, J. E., and Imhof, H-G (1986). Are unilateral right posterior cerebral lesions sufficient to cause prosopagnosia? Clinical and radiological findings in six additional patients.Cortex 22: 243–252.Google Scholar
  105. Landis, M., Regard, M., Blistle, A., and Kleihues, P. (1988). Prosopagnosia and agnosia for noncanonical views.Brain 111: 1287–1297.Google Scholar
  106. Leonard, C. M., Rolls, E. T., Wilson, F. A. W., and Baylis, G. C. (1985). Neurons in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces.Behavioral Brain Research 15: 159–176.Google Scholar
  107. Lewis, S. W. (1987). Brain imaging in a case of Capgras' syndrome.British Journal of Psychiatry 150: 117–121.Google Scholar
  108. Lhermitte, J., Chain, F., Escourolle, R., Ducarne, B., and Pillon, B. (1972). Etude anatomoclinique d'un cas de prosopagnosie.Rev. Neurol. 126: 329–346.Google Scholar
  109. Lipkin, B. (1988). Capgras syndnrome heralding the development of dementia.British Journal of Psychiatry 153: 117–118.Google Scholar
  110. Lissauer, H. (1890). Ein Fall von Seelenblindheiten nebst einen Beitrage zur Theorie derselben.Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankenheiten 21: 222–270 (English translation by Jackson, M., 1988.Cognitive Neuropsychology 5: 157–192).Google Scholar
  111. Luauté, J. P., Bidault, E., Delattre, S., Colomb, H., and Paul, R. (1990). Capgras's syndrome and senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. In Medica, E., Stefanis, C. N., Robavilas, A. D., and Soldatos, C. R. (eds.).Psychiatry: A World Perspective (Vol. 1), Oxford, Amsterdam, pp. 371–376.Google Scholar
  112. MacCallum, W. A. G. (1986). The interplay of organic and psychological factors in delusional misidentification syndromes.Bibliotheca Psychiatrica 164: 92–98.Google Scholar
  113. Macrae, D., and Trolle, E. (1956). The defect of function in visual agnosia.Brain 79: 94–100.Google Scholar
  114. Malloy, P., Cimino, C., and Westlake, R. (1992). Differential diagnosis of primary and secondary Capgras delusions.Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology 5: 83–96.Google Scholar
  115. Mandler, B. A. (1981). Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence.Psychological Review 87: 252–271.Google Scholar
  116. Mayes, A. (1989). Human organic memory disorders: Problems and interpretations. In Crawford, J. R., and Parker, D. M. (eds.),Developments in Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 183–201.Google Scholar
  117. Maylor, E. (1990). Recognizing and naming faces: Aging, memory retrieval, and the tip of the tongue state.Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences 45: 215–226.Google Scholar
  118. McAllister, T. W. (1992). Neuropsychiatric aspects of delusions.Psychiatric Annals 22: 279–285.Google Scholar
  119. McCarthy, R. A., and Warrington, E. K. (1990).Cognitive Neuropsychology: A Clinical Introduction. Academic Press, London.Google Scholar
  120. McClelland, J. L., and Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive model of the effect of context in perception. Part I. An account of basic findings.Psychological Review 88: 375–406.Google Scholar
  121. McClelland, J. L., and Rumelhart, D. E. (1988).Explorations in Parallel Distributed Processing, Bradford Books, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  122. McNeil, J. E., and Warrington, E. K. (1991). Prosopagnosia: A reclassification.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 2: 267–287.Google Scholar
  123. Meadows, J. C. (1974). The anatomical basis of prosopagnosia.Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 37: 489–501.Google Scholar
  124. Meadez, M. F., Martin, R. J., Smyth, K. A., and Whitehouse, P. J. (1992). Disturbances of person identification in Alzheimer's disease: A retrospective study.The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 2: 94–96.Google Scholar
  125. Mishkin, M. (1966). Visual mechanisms beyond the striate cortex. In Russell, R. W. (Ed.), Frontiers in Psychological Psychology, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  126. Mishkin, M., and Appenzeller, T. (1987). The anatomy of memory.Scientific American 256: 80–89.Google Scholar
  127. Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. G., and Macko, K. A. (1981). Object vision and spatial vision: Two cortical pathways.Trends in Neuroscience 6: 414–417.Google Scholar
  128. Newcombe, F., Young, A. W., and De Haan, D. H. F. (1989). Prosopagnosia and object agnosia without covert recognition.Neuropsychologia 27: 179–191.Google Scholar
  129. Ojemann, J. G., Ojemann, G. A., and Lettich, E. (1992). Neuronal activity related to faces and matching in human right nondominant temporal cortex.Brain 115: 1–13.Google Scholar
  130. O'Reilly, R., and Malhotra, L. (1987). Capgras syndrome—An unusual case and discussion of psychodynamic factors.British Journal of Psychiatry 151: 263–265.Google Scholar
  131. Parkin, A. J., and Williamson, P. (1986). Patterns of cerebral dominance in wholistic and featural stages of facial processing. In Ellis, H. D., Jeeves, M. A., Newcombe, F., and Young, A. (eds.),Aspects of Face Processing, Martinus Nijhoff, Dordrecht, Holland, pp. 223–227.Google Scholar
  132. Parkin, A. J., and Williamson, P. (1987). Cerebral lateralisation at different stages of facial processing.Cortex 23: 99–110.Google Scholar
  133. Perrett, D. I., Hietanen, J. K., Oran, M. W., and Benson, P. J. (1992). Organization and functions of cells responsive to faces in the temporal cortex.Philosophila Transactions of the Royal Society of London 335: 23–30.Google Scholar
  134. Perrett, D. I., Mistlin, A. J., Potter, D. D., Smith, P. A. J., and Head, M. A. (1986). In Ellis, H., Jeeves, M. A., Newcomb, F., and Young, A. (eds.),Aspects of Face Processing, Martinus Nijhoff Publishers, Dordrecht, pp. 187–198.Google Scholar
  135. Perrett, D. I., Rolls, E. T., and Caan, W. (1982). Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex.Experimental Brain Research 47: 329–342.Google Scholar
  136. Perrett, D. I., Smith, P. A., Potter, D. D., Mistlin, A. J., Head, A. S., Milner, A. D., and Jeeves, M. A. (1984). Neurones responsive to faces in the temporal cortex: Studies of functional organization, sensitivity to identity and relation to perception.Human Neurobiology 3: 197–208.Google Scholar
  137. Pick, A. (1903). On reduplicative paramnesiaBrain 26: 344–383.Google Scholar
  138. Renault, B., Signoret, J-L, Debruille, B., Breton, F., and Bolgert, F. (1989). Brain potentials reveal covert facial recognition in prosopagnosia.Neuropsychologia 27: 905–912.Google Scholar
  139. Rhodes, G. (1985). Lateralized processes in face recognition.British Journal of Psychology 76: 249–271.Google Scholar
  140. Rizzo, M., Hurtig, R., and Damasio, A. R. (1987). The role of scanpaths in facial recognition and learning,Annals of Neurology 22: 41–45.Google Scholar
  141. Rolls, E. T. (1984). Neurons in the cortex of the temporal lobe and in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces.Human Neurology 3: 209–222.Google Scholar
  142. Rolls, E. T. (1989). The representation and storage of information in neuronal networks in the primate cerebral cortex and hippocampus. In Durbin, R., Miall, C., and Mitchison, G. (eds.),The Computing Neuron, Addison-Wesley, Wokingham, England, pp. 125–159.Google Scholar
  143. Romanik, R. L., and Snow, S. (1984). Two cases of Capgras' syndrome [letter].American Journal of Psychiatry 141: 720.Google Scholar
  144. Sautter, S. W., Briscoe, L., and Farkas, K. (1991). A neuropsychological profile.Neuropsychology 5: 139–150.Google Scholar
  145. Sergent, J. (1984). Configural processing of faces in the left and right cerebral hemispheres.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10: 554–572.Google Scholar
  146. Sergent, J., and Bindra, D. (1981). Differential hemisphere processing of faces: methodological considerations and reinterpretations.Psychological Bulletin 89: 541–554.Google Scholar
  147. Sergent, J., Ohta, S., and MacDonald, B. (1992). Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing.Brain 115: 15–36.Google Scholar
  148. Sergent, J., and Poncet, M. (1990). From covert to overt recognition of faces in a prosopagnosic patient.Brain113: 989–1004.Google Scholar
  149. Sergent, J., and Signoret, J. L. (1992). Functional and anatomical decomposition of face processing: Evidence from prosopagnosia and PET study of normal subjects.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London 335: 55–62.Google Scholar
  150. Sergent, J., and Villemure, J. G. (1989). Prosopagnosia in a right hemispherized patient.Brain 112: 975–995.Google Scholar
  151. Shraberg, D., and Weitzel, W. D. (1979). Prosopagnosia and the Capgras syndrome.Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 40: 313–316.Google Scholar
  152. Signer, S. F. (1987). Capgras' syndrome: The delusion of substitution.Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 48: 147–150.Google Scholar
  153. Signer, S. F. (1992). Capgras syndrome and delusions of reduplication in neurologic disorders.Neuropsychology, Neuropsychiatry, and Behavioral Neurology 5: 138–143.Google Scholar
  154. Silva, J. A., Leong, G. B., and Wine, D. (1993). Misidentification delusions, facial misrecognition, and right brain injury.The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 38: 239–241.Google Scholar
  155. Sinkman, A. M. (1983). The Capgras delusion: A critique of its psychodynamic theories.American Journal of Psychotherapy 273: 428–438.Google Scholar
  156. Smith, A. (1969). Nondominant hemispherectomy.Neurology 19: 442–445.Google Scholar
  157. Spier, S. A. (1992). Capgras syndrome and the delusions of misidentification.Psychiatric Annals 22: 279–285.Google Scholar
  158. Stanhope, N., and Cohen, G. (1993). Retrieval of proper names: Testing the models.British Journal of Psychology 84: 51–65.Google Scholar
  159. Stern, K., and MacNaughton, D. (1945). Capgras syndrome, a peculiar illusionary phenomenon, considered with special reference to Rorschach findings.Psychiatric Quarterly 19: 139–163.Google Scholar
  160. Stuss, D. T., and Benson, D. F. (1984). Neurological studies of the frontal lobes.Psychological Bulletin 95: 3–28.Google Scholar
  161. Szpir, M., with Dunne, T. (atrist) (1992, November–December). Accustomed to your face.American Scientist, pp. 537–539.Google Scholar
  162. Torii, N., and Tami, A. (1985). The problem of prosopagnosia: Report of three cases with occlusion of the right posterior cerebral artery.Journal of Neurology (supplement) 232: 140.Google Scholar
  163. Tranel, D., and Damasio, A. R. (1985). Knowledge without awareness: An autonomic index of facial recognition by prosopagnosics.Science 228: 1453–1454.Google Scholar
  164. Tranel, D., Damasio, A. R., and Damasio, H. (1988). Intact recognition of facial expression, gender and age in patients with impaired recognition of face identity.Neurology 38: 690–696.Google Scholar
  165. Tzavaras, A., Luauté, J. P., and Bidault, J. P. (1986). Face recognition dysfunction and delusional misidentification syndromes (DMS). In Ellis, H. D., Jeeves, M. A., Newcombe, F., and Young A. (eds.),Aspects of Face Processing, Martinus Nijhoff, Dordrecht, Holland, pp. 310–316.Google Scholar
  166. Vogel, R. F. (1974). The Capgras syndrome and its psychopathology.American Journal of Psychiatry 131: 922–924.Google Scholar
  167. Wallace, M. A., and Farah, M. J. (1992). Savings in relearning face-name associates as evidence for “covert recognition,” in prosopagnosia.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 4: 150–154.Google Scholar
  168. Warrington, E. K. (1984).Recognition Memory Test, NFER-Nelson, Windson.Google Scholar
  169. Warrington, E. K., and James, M. (1967). “An experimental investigation of facial recognition in patients with unilateral cerebral lesions,”Cortex 3: 317–326.Google Scholar
  170. Weinstein, E. A., and Burnham, D. L. (1991). Reduplication and the syndrome of Capgras.Psychiatry 54: 78–88.Google Scholar
  171. Yee-Yuen Peng, C., and Campbell, R. (1989). In Crawford, J. R., and Parker, D. M. (eds.),Developments in Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 167–181.Google Scholar
  172. Young, A. W. (1994). Covert recognition. In Farah, M. J., and Ratcliff, G. (eds.),The Neuropsychology of High-Level Vision: Collected Tutorial Essays, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp. 331–358.Google Scholar
  173. Young, A. W., and Bruce, V. (1991). Perceptual categories of the compilation of grandmother.European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 3: 5–49.Google Scholar
  174. Young, A. W. and De Haan, E. H. F. (1988). Boundaries of correct recognition in prosopagnosia.Cognitive Neuropsychology 5: 317–336.Google Scholar
  175. Young, A. W., Ellis, H. D., Szulecka, T. K., and De Pauw, K. W. (1990). Face processing impairments and delusional misidentifications.Behavioral Neurology 3: 153–168.Google Scholar
  176. Young, A. W., Flude, B. M., and Ellis, A. W. (1991). Delusional misidentification in a right hemisphere stroke patient.Behavioral Neurology 4: 81–87.Google Scholar
  177. Young, A. W., Flude, B. M., Hellawell, D. J., and Ellis, A. W. (1994). The nature of semantic priming effects in the recognition of familiar people.British Journal of Psychology 85: 393–411.Google Scholar
  178. Young, A. W., Hay, D. C., and Ellis, A. W. (1985). The faces that launched a thousand slips: Everyday difficulties and errors in recognizing people.British Journal of Psychology 76: 495–523.Google Scholar
  179. Young, A. W., Hellawell, D., and De Haan, E. (1988). Cross-domain semantic priming in normal subjects and a prosopagnosic patient.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 40A: 561–580.Google Scholar
  180. Young, A. W., McWeeny, K. H., Ellis, A. W., and Hay, D. C. (1986). Naming and categorizing faces and written names.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 38A: 297–318.Google Scholar
  181. Young, A. W., McWeeny, K. H., Hay, D. C., and Ellis, A. W. (1986b). Access to identity-specific semantic codes from familiar faces.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 38A: 271–295.Google Scholar
  182. Young, A. W., Reid, I., Wright, S., and Hellawell, D. J. (1994). Face processing impairments in Capgras delusion.British Journal of Psychiatry 162: 695–698.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1996

Authors and Affiliations

  • Elaine Wacholtz
    • 1
  1. 1.School of Human DevelopmentUniversity of Texas at DallasDallas

Personalised recommendations