Educational Psychology Review

, Volume 3, Issue 3, pp 149–210

Dual coding theory and education

  • James M. Clark
  • Allan Paivio
Article

Abstract

Dual coding theory (DCT) explains human behavior and experience in terms of dynamic associative processes that operate on a rich network of modality-specific verbal and nonverbal (or imagery) representations. We first describe the underlying premises of the theory and then show how the basic DCT mechanisms can be used to model diverse educational phenomena. The research demonstrates that concreteness, imagery, and verbal associative processes play major roles in various educational domains: the representation and comprehension of knowledge, learning and memory of school material, effective instruction, individual differences, achievement motivation and test anxiety, and the learning of motor skills. DCT also has important implications for the science and practice of educational psychology — specifically, for educational research and teacher education. We show not only that DCT provides a unified explanation for diverse topics in education, but also that its mechanistic framework accommodates theories cast in terms of strategies and other high-level psychological processes. Although much additional research needs to be done, the concrete models that DCT offers for the behavior and experience of students, teachers, and educational psychologists further our understanding of educational phenomena and strengthen related pedagogical practices.

Key words

imagery verbal processes unified educational theory 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anatasi, A. (1988).Psychological Testing (6th ed.), Macmillan, New York.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, R. C. (1974). Concretization and sentence learning.Journal of Educational Psychology, 66: 179–183.Google Scholar
  3. Anderson, R. C. (1977). The notion of schemata and the educational enterprise. In Anderson, R. C., Spiro, R. J., and Montague, W. E. (eds.),Schooling and the Acquisition of Knowledge, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp. 415–431.Google Scholar
  4. Ash, M. J., and Love-Clark, P. (1985). An historical analysis of the content of Educational Psychology textbooks 1954–1983.Educational Psychologist, 20: 47–55.Google Scholar
  5. Atkinson, R. C. (1975). Mnemotechnics in second-language learning.American Psychologist, 30: 821–828.Google Scholar
  6. Ausubel, D. P. (1960). The use of advance organizers in the learning and retention of meaningful verbal material.Journal of Educational Psychology 51: 267–272.Google Scholar
  7. Baenninger, M., and Newcombe, N. (1988). The role of experience in spatial test performance: A meta-analysis.Sex Roles 20: 327–344.Google Scholar
  8. Baird, D. O. (1931). A vocabulary study of biology notebooks of 50 representative secondary schools in New York State.Journal of Educational Psychology 22: 512–516.Google Scholar
  9. Bandura, A. (1969).Principles of Behavior Modification, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.Google Scholar
  10. Begg, I. (1973). Imagery and integration in the recall of words.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 27: 159–167.Google Scholar
  11. Begg, I., and Paivio, A. (1969). Concreteness and imagery in sentence meaning.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8: 821–827.Google Scholar
  12. Begg, I., Upfold, D., and Wilton, T. D. (1978). Imagery in verbal communication.Journal of Mental Imagery 2: 165–186.Google Scholar
  13. Belka, D. E., and Williams, H. G. (1979). Prediction of later cognitive behavior from early school perceptual-motor, perceptual, and cognitive performances.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49: 131–141.Google Scholar
  14. Bellezza, F. S. (1981). Mnemonic devices: Classification, characteristics, and criteria.Review of Educational Research, 51: 247–275.Google Scholar
  15. Benbow, C. P., and Stanley, J. C. (1983). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability: More facts.Science, 222: 1029–1031.Google Scholar
  16. Benjamin, M., McKeachie, W. J., Lin, Y., and Holinger, D. P. (1981). Test anxiety: Deficits in information processing.Journal of Educational Psychology, 73: 816–824.Google Scholar
  17. Block, J. H., and Tierney, M. L. (1974). An exploration of two correction procedures used in mastery learning approaches to instruction.Journal of Educational Psychology, 66: 962–967.Google Scholar
  18. Bloland, H. G. (1989). Higher education and higher anxiety: Objectivism, relativism, and irony.The Journal of Higher Education, 60: 519–543.Google Scholar
  19. Bousfield, W. K. (1953). The occurrence of clustering in the recall of randomly arranged associates.Journal of General Psychology, 49: 229–240.Google Scholar
  20. Bower, G. H. (1970). Imagery as a relational organizer in associative learning.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9: 529–533.Google Scholar
  21. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory.American Psychologist, 36: 129–148.Google Scholar
  22. Bower, G. H., Black, J. B., and Turner, T. J. (1979). Scripts in memory for text.Cognitive Psychology 11: 177–200.Google Scholar
  23. Bransford J. D., Stein, B. S., Vye, N. J., Franks, J. J., Auble, P. M., Mezynski, K. J., and Perfetto, G. A. (1982). Differences in approaches to learning: An overview.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111: 390–398.Google Scholar
  24. Brophy, J. E. (1979). Teacher behavior and its effects.Journal of Educational Psychology, 71: 733–750.Google Scholar
  25. Brown, G., and Atkins, M. (1988).Effective Teaching in Higher Education, Methuen, London.Google Scholar
  26. Brown, H. I. (1977).Perception, Theory and Commitment: The New Philosophy of Science, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  27. Brown, W. F., and Holtzman, W. H. (1967).Survey of Study Habits and Attitudes: Manual, The Psychological Corporation, New York.Google Scholar
  28. Bruch, M. A., Pearl, L., and Giordano, S. (1986). Differences in the cognitive processes of academically successful and unsuccessful test-anxious students.Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33: 217–219.Google Scholar
  29. Bugelski, B. R. (1970). Words and things and images.American Psychologist 25: 1002–1012.Google Scholar
  30. Burnett, S. A., Lane, D. M., and Dratt, L. M. (1979). Spatial visualization and sex differences in quantitative ability.Intelligence, 3: 345–354.Google Scholar
  31. Butler, S. R., Marsh, H. W., Sheppard, M. J., and Sheppard, J. L. (1982). Early prediction of reading achievement with the Sheppard School Entry Screening Test: A four-year longitudinal study.Journal of Educational Psychology, 74: 280–290.Google Scholar
  32. Buzan, T. (1974).Use Your Head, BBC Books, London.Google Scholar
  33. Cattell, J. M. (1889). Mental association investigated by experiment.Mind, 14: 230–250.Google Scholar
  34. Champion, R. H. (1984). Faculty reported use of research in teacher preparation courses: Six instructional scenarios.Journal of Teacher Education, 35: 9–12.Google Scholar
  35. Chapin, M., and Dyck, D. G. (1976). Persistence in children's reading behavior as a function of N length and attribution retraining.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85: 511–515.Google Scholar
  36. Charters W. W., and Waples, D. (1929).The Commonwealth Teacher-Training Study, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  37. Childs, H. G. (1915). Measurement of the drawing ability of two thousand one hundred and seventy-seven children in Indiana city school systems by a supplemented Thorndike scale.Journal of Educational Psychology, 6: 391–408.Google Scholar
  38. Clark, C. M., Gage, N. L., Marx, R. W., Peterson, P. L., Stayrook, N. G., and Winne, P. H. (1979). A factorial experiment on teacher structuring, soliciting, and reacting.Journal of Educational Psychology, 71: 534–552.Google Scholar
  39. Clark, G. K., and Clark, E. B. (1970).The Art of Lecturing: Some Practical Suggestions, W. Heffer and Sons, Cambridge, England.Google Scholar
  40. Clark, J. M. (1978). Synonymity and concreteness effects on free recall and free association: Implications for a theory of semantic memory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Western Ontario, London, Canada.Google Scholar
  41. Clark, J. M. (1984). Concreteness and semantic repetition effects in free recall: Evidence for dual-coding theory.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 38: 591–598.Google Scholar
  42. Clark, J. M. (1987). Understanding pictures and words: Comment on Potter, Kroll, Yachzel, Carpenter, and Sherman (1986).Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116: 307–309.Google Scholar
  43. Clark, J. M. (1988, June).Accessing knowledge from pictures and words. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Canadian Psychological Association, Montreal, Quebec.Google Scholar
  44. Clark, J. M. (1989). Reconciliation of data on aptitude test scores: and university grades of teachers.Journal of Teacher Education, 40: 49–52.Google Scholar
  45. Clark, J. M., and Campbell, J. I. D. (1991). Integrated versus modular theories of number skills and acalculia.Brain and Cognition (in press).Google Scholar
  46. Clark, J. M., and Johnson, C. J. (unpublished). Toward a developmental model for instance and superordinate naming of pictures.Google Scholar
  47. Clark, J. M., and Paivio, A. (1984, May).Associative mechanisms in cognition. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Canadian Psychological Association, Ottawa, Canada.Google Scholar
  48. Clark, J. M., and Paivio, A. (1987). A dual coding perspective on encoding processes. In McDaniel, M. A., and Pressley, M. (eds.),Imagery and Related Mnemonic Processes: Theories, Individual Differences, and Applications, Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 5–33.Google Scholar
  49. Clark, J. M., and Paivio, A. (1989). Observational and theoretical terms in psychology: A cognitive perspective on scientific language.American Psychologist, 44: 500–512.Google Scholar
  50. Clark, J. M., and Paivio, A. (1989, June). Arousal of affect by pictures and words. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Canadian Psychological Association, Halifax, Canada.Google Scholar
  51. Clifford, G. J. (1973). A history of the impact of research on teaching. In Travers, R. M. W. (ed.),Second Handbook of Research on Teaching, Rand McNally, Chicago, pp. 1–46.Google Scholar
  52. Coates, T. J., and Thoresen, C. E. (1976). Teacher anxiety: A review with recommendations.Review of Educational Research, 46: 159–184.Google Scholar
  53. Collins, A. M., and Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing.Psychological Review, 82: 407–428.Google Scholar
  54. Corkill, A. J., Glover, J. A., and Bruning, R. H. (1988). Advance organizers: Concrete versus abstract.Journal of Educational Research, 82: 76–81.Google Scholar
  55. Craighead, W. E., Kazdin, A. E., and Mahoney, M. J. (1976).Behavior Modification: Principles, Issues, and Applications, Houghton Mifflin, Boston.Google Scholar
  56. Craik, F., and Lockhart, R. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11: 671–684.Google Scholar
  57. Cramer, P. (1968).Word Association, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  58. Cromer, R. F. (1983). Hierarchical planning disability in the drawings and constructions of a special group of severely aphasic children.Brain and Cognition, 2: 144–164.Google Scholar
  59. D'Agostino, P. R., O'Neill, B. J., and Paivio, A. (1977). Memory for pictures and words as a function of levels of processing: Depth or dual coding?Memory & Cognition, 5: 252–256.Google Scholar
  60. Dansereau, D. F., Collins, K. W., McDonald, B. A., Holley, C. D., Garland, J., Diekhoff, G., and Evans, S. H. (1979). Development and evaluation of a learning strategy training program.Journal of Educational Psychology, 71: 64–73.Google Scholar
  61. Das, J. P., Kirby, J., and Jarman, R. F. (1975). Simultaneous and successive syntheses: An alternative model for cognitive abilities.Psychological Bulletin, 82: 87–103.Google Scholar
  62. Davitz, J. R. (1969).The Language of Emotion, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  63. Dean, R. S., and Kulhavy, R. W. (1981). Influence of spatial organization in prose learning.Journal of Educational Psychology, 73: 57–64.Google Scholar
  64. deGroot, A. M. B. (1989). Representational aspects of word imageability and word frequency as assessed through word association.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15: 824–845.Google Scholar
  65. Denckla, M. B., Rudel, R. G., and Broman, M. (1981). Tests that discriminate between dyslexic and other learning-disabled boys.Brain and Language, 13: 118–129.Google Scholar
  66. Denis, M. (1982). Imaging while reading text: A study of individual differences.Memory & Cognition, 10: 540–545.Google Scholar
  67. Denis, M. (1984). Imagery and prose: A critical review of research on adults and children.Text, 4: 381–401.Google Scholar
  68. Desrochers, A., and Begg, I. (1987). A theoretical of encoding and retrieval processes in the use of imagery-based mnemonic techniques: The special case of the keyword method. In McDaniel, M. A., and Pressley, M. (eds.),Imagery and Related Mnemonic Processes: Theories, Individual Differences, and Applications, Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 56–77.Google Scholar
  69. Dillon, R. F., and Sternberg, R. J. (1986).Cognition and Instruction, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  70. Donald, J. G. (1986). Knowledge and the university curriculum.Higher Education, 15: 267–282.Google Scholar
  71. Dunkin, M. J., and Doenau, S. J. (1980). A replication study of unique and joint contributions to variance in student achievement.Journal of Educational Psychology 72: 394–403.Google Scholar
  72. Eastwood, J. (1988). Qualitative research: An additional research methodology for speech pathology?British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 23: 171–184.Google Scholar
  73. Ebmeier, H., and Good, T. L. (1979). The effects of instructing teachers about good teaching on the mathematics achievement of fourth grade students.American Educational Research Journal, 16: 1–16.Google Scholar
  74. Engelkamp, J. (1990). Memory of action events: Some implications for memory theory and for imagery. In Cornoldi, C., and McDaniel, M. A. (eds.),Imagery and Cognition, Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 181–219.Google Scholar
  75. Ernest, C. H., and Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal associative latencies as a function of imagery ability.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 25: 83–90.Google Scholar
  76. Eylon, B., and Linn, M. C. (1988). Learning and instruction: An examination of four research perspectives in science education.Review of Educational Research, 58: 251–301.Google Scholar
  77. Feltz, D. L., and Landers, D. M. (1983). The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance: A meta-analysis.Journal of Sport Psychology, 5: 25–57.Google Scholar
  78. Finley, F. N. (1983). Science processes.Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 20: 47–54.Google Scholar
  79. Fischman, M. G., Christina, R. W., and Vercruyssen, M. J. (1981). Retention and transfer of motor skills: A review for the practitioner.Quest, 33: 181–194.Google Scholar
  80. Fisher, K. M. (1988). Depression in free-word-association responses at adolescence.Journal of Educational Psychology, 80: 247–250.Google Scholar
  81. Fitts, P. M., and Switzer, G. (1962). Cognitive aspects of information processing: I. The familiarity of S-R sets and subsets.Journal of Experimental Psychology 63: 321–329.Google Scholar
  82. Fleishman, E. A., and Rich, S. (1963). Role of kinesthetic and spatial-visual abilities in perceptual-motor learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66: 6–11.Google Scholar
  83. Flesch, R. (1950). Measuring the level of abstraction.Journal of Applied Psychology, 34: 384–390.Google Scholar
  84. Frey, P. W., Leonard, D. W., and Beatty, W. W. (1975). Student ratings of instruction: Validation research.American Educational Research Journal, 12: 435–447.Google Scholar
  85. Gagne, E. D. (1985).The Cognitive Psychology of School Learning, Little, Brown and Company, Boston.Google Scholar
  86. Gagne, R. M. (1966). Elementary science: A new scheme of instruction.Science, 151: 49–53.Google Scholar
  87. Gagne, R. M. (1977).The Conditions of Learning (3d ed), Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.Google Scholar
  88. Gardner, M. F. (1979).Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test, Academic Therapy Publications, Novato, California.Google Scholar
  89. Gauld, C. F., and Hukins, A. A. (1980). Scientific attitudes: A review.Studies in Science Education, 7: 129–161.Google Scholar
  90. Gentner, D., and Stevens, A. L. (eds.) (1983).Mental Models, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  91. Gilliland, J. (1972).Readability, Hodder and Stoughton, London.Google Scholar
  92. Glaser, R. (1981). The future of testing: A research agenda for cognitive psychology and psychometrics.American Psychologist, 36: 923–936.Google Scholar
  93. Glenberg, A. M., Meyer, M., and Lindem, K. (1987). Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension.Journal of Memory and Language 26: 69–83.Google Scholar
  94. Gliessman, D. H., Pugh, R. C., Dowden, D. E., and Hutchins, T. E. (1988). Variables influencing the acquisition of a generic teaching skill.Review of Educational Research, 58: 25–46.Google Scholar
  95. Goldschmid, M. L. (1967). Prediction of college majors by personality tests.Journal of Counseling Psychology, 14: 302–308.Google Scholar
  96. Goss, S., Hall, C., Buckolz, E., and Fishburne, G. (1986). Imagery ability and the acquisition and retention of movements.Memory & Cognition, 14: 469–477.Google Scholar
  97. Guerin, G. R. (1979). Regular teacher concerns with mainstreamed learning handicapped children.Psychology in the Schools, 16: 543–545.Google Scholar
  98. Guilford, J. P. (1967).The Nature of Human Intelligence, McGraw-Hill, New York.Google Scholar
  99. Guri-Rozenblit, S. (1989). Effects of a tree diagram on students' comprehension of main ideas in an expository text with multiple themes.Reading Research Quarterly, 24: 236–247.Google Scholar
  100. Halliday, M. A. K., and Hasan, R. (1976).Cohesion in English, Longman, London.Google Scholar
  101. Hembree, R. (1988). Correlates, causes, effects, and treatment of test anxiety.Review of Educational Research 58: 47–77.Google Scholar
  102. Heshusias, L. (1988). The arts, science, and the study of exceptionality.Exceptional Children, 55: 60–65.Google Scholar
  103. Hidi, S., and Baird, W. (1988). Strategies for increasing text-based interest and students' recall of expository texts.Reading Research Quarterly, 23: 465–483.Google Scholar
  104. Hiller, J. H., Fischer, G. A., and Kaess, W. (1969). A computer investigation of verbal characteristics of effective classroom lecturing.American Educational Research Journal, 6: 661–675.Google Scholar
  105. Hodson, D. (1985). Philosophy of science, science, and science education.Studies in Science Education, 12: 25–57.Google Scholar
  106. Holley, C. D., and Dansereau, D. F. (1984).Spatial Learning Strategies: Techniques, Applications, and Related Issues, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  107. Hyde, J. S. (1981). How large are cognitive gender differences? A meta-analysis using w2 and d.American Psychologist, 36: 892–901.Google Scholar
  108. Hyman, R. (1953). Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45: 188–196.Google Scholar
  109. Jackson, D. N. (1977).Jackson Vocational Interest Survey: Manual, Research Psychologists Press, Port Huron, Michigan.Google Scholar
  110. Jackson, D. N. (1984).Multidimensional Aptitude Battery (MAB): Manual, Research Psychologists Press, Port Huron, Michigan.Google Scholar
  111. Jensen, A. R. (1987). Individual differences in the Hick paradigm. In Vernon, P.A. (ed.),Speed of Information-Processing and Intelligence, Ablex, Norwood, New Jersey, pp. 101–175.Google Scholar
  112. Johnson, C. J., and Clark, J. M. (1988). Children's picture naming difficulty and errors: Effects of age of acquisition, uncertainty, and name generality.Applied Psycholinguistics, 9: 351–365.Google Scholar
  113. Johnson, P. E. (1964). Associative meaning of concepts in physics.Journal of Educational Psychology, 55: 84–88.Google Scholar
  114. Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983).Mental Models: Toward a Cognitive Science of Language, Inference, and Consciousness, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  115. Johnston, J. M. (1975).Behavior Research and Technology in Higher Education, Charles C. Thomas, Springfield, Illinois.Google Scholar
  116. Jones, G. V. (1985). Deep dyslexia, imageability, and ease of predication.Brain and Language, 24: 1–19.Google Scholar
  117. Jorgenson, C. C., and Kintsch, W. (1973). The role of imagery in the evaluation of sentences.Cognitive Psychology, 4: 110–116.Google Scholar
  118. Kagan, D. M. (1988). Research on the supervision of counselors- and teachers-in-training: Linking two bodies of literature.Review of Educational Research, 58: 1–24.Google Scholar
  119. Katz, A. N. (1976). Verbal concept identification: Disentangling the dominance effect. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Western Ontario.Google Scholar
  120. Katz, A. N., Paivio, A., Marschark, M., and Clark, J. M. (1988). Norms for 204 literary and 260 nonliterary metaphors on 10 psychological dimensions.Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 3: 191–214.Google Scholar
  121. Kaufman, A. S., and Kaufman, N. L. (1983).Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, Minnesota.Google Scholar
  122. Kellogg, R. T. (1988). Attentional overload and writing performance: Effects of rough draft and outline strategies.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14: 355–365.Google Scholar
  123. Khan, M. (1989. Memorial effects of importance and taxonomic frequency in schematic and categorical organizations of knowledge. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Western Ontario, London, Canada.Google Scholar
  124. Khan, M., and Paivio, A. (1988). Memory for schematic and categorical information: A replication and extension of Rabinowitz and Mandler (1983).Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14: 558–561.Google Scholar
  125. Kimura, D. (1987). Are men's and women's brains really different?Canadian Psychology, 28: 133–147.Google Scholar
  126. Kimura, D., and Archibald, Y. (1974). Motor functions of the left hemisphere.Brain, 97: 337–350.Google Scholar
  127. Kintsch, W. (1974).The Representation of Meaning in Memory, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  128. Kintsch, W., and Keenan, J. (1973). Reading rate and retention as a function of the number of propositions in the base structure of sentences.Cognitive Psychology, 5: 257–274.Google Scholar
  129. Kintsch, W., and van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production.Psychological Review, 85: 363–394.Google Scholar
  130. Kirschner, P. A., and Meester, M. A. M. (1988). The laboratory in higher science education: Problems, premises, and objectives.Higher Education, 17: 81–98.Google Scholar
  131. Klare, G. R. (1963).The Measurement of Readability, Iowa State University Press, Ames, Iowa.Google Scholar
  132. Klare, G. R. (1974/1975). Assessing readability.Reading Research Quarterly, 10: 62–102.Google Scholar
  133. Klatzky, R. L., McCloskey, B., Doherty, S., Pellegrino, J., and Smith, T. (1987). Knowledge about hand shaping and knowledge about objects.Journal of Motor Behavior, 19: 187–213.Google Scholar
  134. Klatzky, R. L., Pellegrino, J. W., McCloskey, B. P., and Doherty, S. (1989). Can you squeeze a tomato? The role of motor representations in semantic sensibility judgments.Journal of Memory and Language, 28: 56–77.Google Scholar
  135. Klee, H., and Eysenck, M. W. (1973). Comprehension of abstract and concrete sentences.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12: 522–529.Google Scholar
  136. Koch, S. (1981). The nature and limits of psychological knowledge: Lessons of a century qua “science.”American Psychologist 36: 257–269.Google Scholar
  137. Kornblum, S. (1965). Response competition and/or inhibition in two-choice reaction time.Psychonomic Science, 2: 55–56.Google Scholar
  138. Kuhn, T. S. (1962).The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  139. Kulhavy, R. W., and Kardash, C. A. M. (1988). Studying study: An analysis of instructional encoding behavior.Educational and Psychological Research, 8: 25–38.Google Scholar
  140. Lachman, R. (1973). Uncertainty effects on time to access the internal lexicon.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 99: 199–208.Google Scholar
  141. Lang, P. J. (1979a). Language, image, and emotion. In Pliner, P., Blankstein, K. R., and Spigel, I. M. (eds.),Perception of Emotion in Self and Others, Plenum, New York, pp. 107–117.Google Scholar
  142. Lang, P. J. (1979b). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery.Psychophysiology, 16: 495–512.Google Scholar
  143. Lang, P. J. (1984). Cognition in emotion: Concept and action. In Izard, C. E., Kagan, J., and Zajonc, R. B. (eds.),Emotions, Cognition, and Behavior, Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 192–226.Google Scholar
  144. Lawton, D. (1968).Social Class, Language and Education, Routledge & Kegan Paul, London.Google Scholar
  145. Levin, J. R., and Berry, J. K. (1980). Children's learning of all the news that's fit to picture.Educational Communication and Technology, 28: 177–185.Google Scholar
  146. Levin, J. R., and Pressley, M. (1985). Mnemonic vocabulary instruction: What's fact, what's fiction. In Dillon, R. F. (ed.),Individual Differences in Cognition, Vol. 2, Academic Press, New York, pp. 145–172.Google Scholar
  147. Lewis, R. S., and Christiansen, L. (1989). Intrahemispheric sex differences in the functional representation of language and praxic function in normal individuals.Brain and Cognition, 9: 238–243.Google Scholar
  148. Linn, M. C., and Hyde, J. S. (1989). Gender, mathematics, and science.Educational Researcher, 18: 17–19, 22–27.Google Scholar
  149. Lohman, D. F., and Kyllonen, P. C. (1983). Individual differences in solution strategy on spatial tasks. In Dillon, R. F., and Schmeck, R. R. (eds.),Individual Differences in Cognition, Vol. 1, Academic Press, New York, pp. 105–135.Google Scholar
  150. Long, S. A., Winograd, P. N., and Bridge, C. A. (1989). The effects of reader and text characteristics on imagery reported during and after reading.Reading Research Quarterly, 24: 353–372.Google Scholar
  151. Luria, A. R. (1961).The Role of Speech in the Regulation of Normal and Abnormal Behavior, Pergamon, London.Google Scholar
  152. Maccoby, E. E. (1980).Social Development: Psychological Development and the Parent-Child Relationship, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, New York.Google Scholar
  153. Maccoby, E. E., and Jacklin, C. N. (1974).The Psychology of Sex Differences, Stanford University Press, Stanford, California.Google Scholar
  154. Mandler, G. (1958). Autonomic feedback: The perception of autonomic activity.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 56: 367–373.Google Scholar
  155. Mandler, G. (1975).Mind and Emotion, Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  156. Mandler, G. (1984).Mind and Body: Psychology of Emotion and Stress, W. W. Norton, New York.Google Scholar
  157. Mandler, J. M. (1984).Stories, Scripts, and Scenes: Aspects of Schema Theory, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  158. Marschark, M. (1985). Imagery and organization in the recall of prose.Journal of Memory and Language, 24: 734–745.Google Scholar
  159. Marschark, M., and Hunt, R. R. (1989). A reexamination of the role of imagery in learning and memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15: 710–720.Google Scholar
  160. Marschark, M., and Paivio, A. (1977). Integrative processing of concrete and abstract sentences.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16: 217–231.Google Scholar
  161. Marschark, M., Richman, C. L., Yuille, J. C., and Hunt, R. R. (1987). The role of imagery in memory: On shared and distinctive information.Psychological Bulletin, 102: 28–41.Google Scholar
  162. Marsh, H. W. (1982). Validity of students' evaluations of college teaching: A multitrait-multimethod analysis.Journal of Educational Psychology, 74: 264–279.Google Scholar
  163. Mayer, R. E. (1987).Educational Psychology: A Cognitive Approach, Little, Brown, Boston.Google Scholar
  164. Mayer, R. E. (1989). Models for understanding.Review of Educational Research, 59: 43–64.Google Scholar
  165. McAshan, H. H. (1974).The goals Approach to Performance Objectives, W. B. Saunders, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
  166. McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. W., and Lowell, E. L. (1953).The Achievement Motive, Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.Google Scholar
  167. McKenna, M. C., and Robinson, R. D. (1980).An Introduction to the Cloze Procedure: An Annotated Bibliography, International Reading Association, Newark, Delaware.Google Scholar
  168. McMurry, F. M. (1909).How to Study and Teaching How to Study, Houghton Mifflin, New York.Google Scholar
  169. Meichenbaum, D. (1976). Toward a cognitive theory of self-control. In Schwartz, G. E., and Shapiro, D. (eds.),Consciousness of Self-Regulation: Advances in Research, Vol. 1, Plenum, New York, pp. 223–260.Google Scholar
  170. Mercer, C. D., and Mercer, A. R. (1981).Teaching Students with Learning Problems, Charles E. Merrill, Columbus, Ohio.Google Scholar
  171. Meyer, B. J. F. (1977). What is remembered from prose: A function of passage structure. In Freedle, R. O. (ed.),Advances in Discourse Production (Vol. 1): Discourse Production and Comprehension, Ablex, Norwood, New Jersey, pp. 307–336.Google Scholar
  172. Miller, S. (1970). Prediction of mediated paired-associate learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86: 131–132.Google Scholar
  173. Moore, B. M. (1988). Achievement in basic math skills for low performing students: A study of teachers' affect and CAI.Journal of Experimental Education, 57: 38–44.Google Scholar
  174. Moss, C. J. (1982). Academic achievement and individual differences in the learning processes of basic skills students in the university.Applied Psychological Measurement, 6: 291–296.Google Scholar
  175. Murray, D. J. (1982). Rated associability and episodic memory.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36: 420–434.Google Scholar
  176. Murray, H. G. (1983a). Low-inference classroom teaching behaviors in relation to six measures of college teaching effectiveness. InProceedings of the Conference on the Evaluation and Improvement of University Teaching: The Canadian Experience, Montebello, Quebec.Google Scholar
  177. Murray, H. G. (1983b). Low-inference classroom teaching behaviors and student ratings of college teaching effectiveness.Journal of Educational Psychology, 75: 138–149.Google Scholar
  178. Murray, H. G. (1985). Classroom teaching behaviors related to college teaching effectiveness. In Donald, J. G., and Sullivan, A. M. (eds.),Using Research to Improve Teaching, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp. 21–34.Google Scholar
  179. Nagy, P. (ed.) (1984).The Representation of Cognitive Structure, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
  180. Naveh-Benjamin, M., McKeachie, W. J., Lin, Y., and Tucker, D. G. (1986). Inferring students' cognitive structures and their development using the “Ordered Tree Technique.”Journal of Educational Psychology, 78: 130–140.Google Scholar
  181. Noterdaeme, M., Amorosa, H., Ploog, M., and Scheimann, G. (1988). Quantitative and qualitative aspects of associated movements in children with specific developmental speech and language disorders and in normal pre-school children.Journal of Human Movement Studies, 15: 151–169.Google Scholar
  182. Novak, J. D., and Gowin, D. B. (1984).Learning How to Learn, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  183. Nystrom, N. J., King, J. A., and Wimpelberg, R. K. (1984). Teacher education in private colleges and universities: Scholarship and teaching.Journal of Teacher Education, 35: 42–45.Google Scholar
  184. O'Neill, B. J. (1972). Defineability as an index of word meaning.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1: 287–298.Google Scholar
  185. O'Neill, B. J., and Paivio, A. (1978). Some consequences of violating selection restrictions in concrete and abstract sentences.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 32: 3–18.Google Scholar
  186. Ozols, E. J., and Rourke, B. P. (1988). Characteristics of young learning-disabled children classified according to patterns of academic achievement: Auditory-perceptual and visual-perceptual abilities.Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 17: 44–52.Google Scholar
  187. Paivio, A. (1963). Learning of adjective-noun paired-associates as a function of adjective-noun word order and noun abstractness.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 17: 370–379.Google Scholar
  188. Paivio, A. (1965). Personality and audience influence. In Maher, B. A. (ed.),Progress in Experimental Personality Research, Vol. 2, Academic Press, New York, pp. 127–173.Google Scholar
  189. Paivio, A. (1966). Latency of verbal associations and imagery to noun stimuli as a function of abstractness and generality.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 20: 378–387.Google Scholar
  190. Paivio, A. (1968). A factor-analytic study of word attributes and verbal learning.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7: 41–49.Google Scholar
  191. Paivio, A. (1969). Mental imagery in associative learning and memory.Psychological Review, 76: 241–263.Google Scholar
  192. Paivio, A. (1971).Imagery and Verbal Processes, Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, New York (Reprinted 1979, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey).Google Scholar
  193. Paivio, A. (1975a). Neomentalism.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 29: 263–291.Google Scholar
  194. Paivio, A. (1975b). Coding distinctions and repetition effects in memory. In Bower, G. H. (ed.),The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 9, Academic Press, New York, pp. 179–214.Google Scholar
  195. Paivio, A. (1975c). Imagery and synchronic thinking.Canadian Psychological Review, 16: 147–163.Google Scholar
  196. Paivio, A. (1978). Mental comparisons involving abstract attributes.Memory & Cognition, 6: 199–208.Google Scholar
  197. Paivio, A. (1983a). The minds eye in arts and science.Poetics, 12: 1–18.Google Scholar
  198. Paivio, A. (1983b). The empirical case for dual coding theory. In Yuille, J. C. (ed.),Imagery, Memory and Cognition: Essays in Honor of Allan Paivio, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp. 307–332.Google Scholar
  199. Paivio, A. (1985). Cognitive and motivational functions of imagery in human performance.Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences, 10: 22–28.Google Scholar
  200. Paivio, A. (1986).Mental Representations: A Dual-Coding Approach, Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  201. Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status.Canadian Journal of Psychology.Google Scholar
  202. Paivio, A., and Begg, I. (1971a). Imagery and associative overlap in short-term memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89: 40–45.Google Scholar
  203. Paivio, A., and Begg, I. (1971b). Imagery and comprehension latencies as a function of sentence concreteness and structure.Perception and Psychophysics, 10: 408–412.Google Scholar
  204. Paivio, A., and Begg, I. (1981).Psychology of Language, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  205. Paivio, A., and Clark, J. M. (1986). The role of topic and vehicle imagery in metaphor comprehension.Communication & Cognition 19: 367–388.Google Scholar
  206. Paivio, A., and Clark, J. M. (1991). Static versus dynamic imagery. In Cornoldi, C., and McDaniel, M. (eds.),Imagery and Cognition, Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 221–245.Google Scholar
  207. Paivio, A., Clark, J. M., Digdon, N., and Bons, T. (1989). Referential processing: Reciprocity and correlates of naming and imaging.Memory & Cognition, 17: 163–174.Google Scholar
  208. Paivio, A., Clark, J. M., and Khan, M. (1988a). Effects of concreteness and semantic relatedness on composite imagery and cued recall.Memory & Cognition, 16: 422–430.Google Scholar
  209. Paivio, A., Clark, J. M., and Lambert, W. E. (1988b). Bilingual dual coding theory and semantic repetition effects on recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 14: 163–172.Google Scholar
  210. Paivio, A., and Desrochers, A. (1980). A dual-coding approach to bilingual memory.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 34: 390–401.Google Scholar
  211. Paivio, A., and Desrochers, A. (1981). Mnemonic techniques in second-language learning.Journal of Educational Psychology, 73: 780–795.Google Scholar
  212. Paivio, A., and Harshman, R. A. (1983). Factor analysis of a questionnaire on imagery and verbal habits and skills.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 37: 461–483.Google Scholar
  213. Paivio, A., and Lambert, W. E. (1959). Measures and correlates of audience anxiety (“stage fright”).Journal of Personality, 27: 1–17.Google Scholar
  214. Paivio, A., and Lambert, W. (1981). Dual coding and bilingual memory.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20: 532–539.Google Scholar
  215. Paivio, A., and Marschark, M. (1980). Comparative judgments of animal intelligence and pleasantness.Memory & Cognition, 8: 39–48.Google Scholar
  216. Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., and Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns.Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph Supplement, 76 (1, Pt. 2): 1–25.Google Scholar
  217. Pellegrino, J. (1987). Measuring versus understanding individual differences in cognitive abilities. In Modgil, S., and Modgil, C. (eds.),Arthur Jensen: Consensus and Controversy, Falmer Press, New York, pp. 267–277.Google Scholar
  218. Potter, M. C., Kroll, J. F., Yachzel, B., Carpenter E., and Sherman, J. (1986). Pictures in sentences: Understanding without words.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115: 281–294.Google Scholar
  219. Potter, M. C., and Kroll, J. F. (1987). Conceptual representation of pictures and words: Reply to Clark.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116: 310–311.Google Scholar
  220. Preece, P. F. W. (1976). Mapping cognitive structure: A comparison of methods.Journal of Educational Psychology, 68: 1–8.Google Scholar
  221. Pressley, M., Borkowski, J. G., and O'Sullivan, J. (1985). Children's metamemory and the teaching of memory strategies. In Forrest-Pressley, D. L., MacKinnon, G. E., and Waller, T. G. (eds.),Metacognition, Cognition, and Human Performance, Academic Press, New York, pp. 111–153.Google Scholar
  222. Pressley, M., Goodchild, F., Fleet, J., Zajchowski, R., and Evans, E. D. (1989a). The challenges of classroom strategy instruction.The Elementary School Journal, 89: 301–342.Google Scholar
  223. Pressley, M., Johnson, C. J., Symons, S., McGoldrick, J., and Kurita, J. (1989b). Strategies that improve children's memory and comprehension of what is read.Elementary School Journal, 90: 3–32.Google Scholar
  224. Pressley, M., McDaniel, M. A., Turnure, J. E., Wood, E., and Ahmad, M. (1987). Generation and precision of elaboration: Effects on intentional and incidental learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13: 291–300.Google Scholar
  225. Pressley, M., Symons, S., Snyder, B. L., and Cariglia-Bull, T. (1989c). Strategy instruction research comes of age.Learning Disability Quarterly, 12: 16–30.Google Scholar
  226. Rabinowitz, M., and Mandler, J. M. (1983). Organization and information retrieval.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9: 430–439.Google Scholar
  227. Raygor, A. L. (1970).Study Skills Test: Examiner's Manual, McGraw-Hill, Monterey, California.Google Scholar
  228. Reif, F., and Heller, J. I. (1982). Knowledge structure and problem solving in physics.Educational Psychologist, 17: 102–127.Google Scholar
  229. Resnick, L. B. (1981). Social assumptions as a context for science: Some reflections on psychology and education.Educational Psychologist, 16: 1–10.Google Scholar
  230. Reynolds, A., and Paivio, A. (1968). Cognitive and emotional determinants of speech.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 22: 164–175.Google Scholar
  231. Richardson, J. T. E. (1980). Concreteness, imagery, and semantic categorization.Journal of Mental Imagery, 4: 51–58.Google Scholar
  232. Riding, R. J., and Calvey, I. (1981). The assessment of verbal-imagery learning styles and their effect on the recall of concrete and abstract prose passages by 11-year-old children.British Journal of Psychology, 72: 59–64.Google Scholar
  233. Robinson, F. P. (1970).Effective Study (4th ed.), Harper & Row, New York.Google Scholar
  234. Robinson, N. M., and Robinson, H. B. (1976).The Mentally Retarded Child: A Psychological Approach (2nd ed), McGraw-Hill, New York.Google Scholar
  235. Roediger, H. L. (1980). The effectiveness of four mnemonics in ordering recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6: 558–567.Google Scholar
  236. Ronning, R. R., Conoley, J. C., and Glover, J. A. (1987). Introduction: The implications of cognitive psychology for testing. In Ronning, R. R., Conoley, J. C., Glover, J. A., and Witt, J. C. (eds.),The Influence of Cognitive Psychology on Testing: Buros-Nebraska Symposium on Measurement and Testing, Vol. 3, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp. 1–8.Google Scholar
  237. Rosenshine, B., and Furst, N. F. (1971). Research on teacher performance criteria. In Smith, B. O. (ed.),Research in Teacher Education: A Symposium, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey, pp. 37–72.Google Scholar
  238. Rosenshine, B., and Martin, M. (1974). Teacher education and teacher behavior: Comments on the state-of-the-research.Educational Researcher 3: 11–12.Google Scholar
  239. Royce, J. R. (1982). Philosophical issues, division 24, and the future.American Psychologist 37: 258–266.Google Scholar
  240. Rumberger, R. W. (1987). High school dropouts: A review of issues and evidence.Review of Educational Research, 57: 101–121.Google Scholar
  241. Sadoski, M. (1983). An exploratory study of the relationships between reported imagery and the comprehension and recall of a story.Reading Research Quarterly, 19: 110–123.Google Scholar
  242. Sadoski, M. (1984). Text structure, imagery, and affect in the recall of a story by children. In Niles, J. A., and Harris, L. A. (eds.),Changing Perspectives in Research in Reading/Language Processing and Instruction, 33d Yearbook of the National Reading Conference, National Reading Conference, Washington, D.C., pp. 48–53.Google Scholar
  243. Sadoski, M. (1985). The natural use of imagery in story comprehension and recall: Replication and extension.Reading Research Quarterly, 20: 658–667.Google Scholar
  244. Sadoski, M., and Goetz, E. T. (1985). Relationships between affect, imagery, and importance ratings for segments of a story. In Niles, J. A., and Lalik, R. (eds.),Issues in Literacy: A Research Perspective, 34th Yearbook of the National Reading Conference, National Reading Conference, Washington, D.C., pp. 180–185.Google Scholar
  245. Salvia, J., and Ysseldyke, J. E. (1988).Assessment in Special and Remedial Education (4th ed.), Houghton-Mifflin, Boston.Google Scholar
  246. Schibeci, R. A. (1981). Do teachers rate science attitude objectives as highly as cognitive objectives?Joural of Research in Science Teaching, 18: 69–72.Google Scholar
  247. Schibeci, R. A. (1984). Attitudes to science: An update.Studies in Science Education, 11: 26–59.Google Scholar
  248. Schibeci, R. A., and McGaw, B. (1981). Empirical validation of the conceptual structure of a test of science-related attitudes.Educational and Psychological Measurement, 41: 1195–1201.Google Scholar
  249. Schmeck, R. R. (1983). Learning styles of college students. In Dillon, R. F., and Schmeck, R. R. (eds.), Individual Differences in Cognition: Vol. 1, Academic Press, New York, pp. 233–279.Google Scholar
  250. Schmeck, R. R., and Ribich, F. D. (1978). Construct validation of the inventory of learning processes.Applied Psychological Measurement, 2: 551–562.Google Scholar
  251. Schmeck, R. R., Ribich, F. D., and Ramanaiah, N. (1977). Development of a self-report inventory for assessing individual differences in learning processes.Applied Psychological Measurement, 1: 413–431.Google Scholar
  252. Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning.Psychological Review, 82: 225–260.Google Scholar
  253. Schwartz, G. E., Ahern, G. L., and Brown, S. (1979). Lateralized facial muscle response to positive and negative emotional stimuli.Psychophysiology 16: 561–571.Google Scholar
  254. Schwartz, G. E., Davidson, R. J., and Goleman, D. J. (1978). Patterning of cognitive and somatic processes in the self-regulation of anxiety: Effects of mediation versus exercise.Psychosomatic Medicine, 40: 321–328.Google Scholar
  255. Shallice, T. (1978). The dominant action system: An information processing approach to consciousness. In Pope, K. S., and Singer, J. L. (eds.),The Stream of Consciousness: Scientific Investigations into the Flow of Human Experience, Plenum, New York, pp. 117–157.Google Scholar
  256. Shavelson, R. J. (1974). Methods for examining representations of a subject-matter structure in a student's memory.Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 11: 231–249.Google Scholar
  257. Shepard, R. N. (1978). Externalization of mental images and the act of creation. In Randhawa, B. S., and Coffman, W. E. (eds.),Visual Learning, Thinking, and Communication, Academic Press, New York, pp. 133–189.Google Scholar
  258. Simmons, D. C., Griffin, C. C., and Kameenui, E. J. (1988a). Effects of teacher-constructed pre- and post-graphic organizer instruction on sixth-grade science students' comprehension and recall.Journal of Educational Research, 82: 15–21.Google Scholar
  259. Simner, M. L. (1989). Predictive validity of an abbreviated version of the Printing Performance School Readiness Test.Journal of School Psychology, 27: 189–195.Google Scholar
  260. Singer, J. L. (1966).Daydreaming: An Introduction to the Experimental Study of Inner Experience, Random House, New York.Google Scholar
  261. Sisson, L. A., Kilwein, M. L., and van Hasselt, V. B. (1988). A graduated guidance procedure for teaching self-dressing skills to multihandicapped children.Research in Developmental Disabilities, 9: 419–432.Google Scholar
  262. Snowling, M., Van Wagtendonk, B., and Stafford, C. (1988). Object naming deficits in developmental dyslexia.Journal of Research in Reading, 11: 67–85.Google Scholar
  263. Sosniak, L. A., and Ethington, C. A. (1989). School quality and high school seniors' attitudes toward academic coursework.Journal of Educational Research, 82: 113–119.Google Scholar
  264. Starch, D. (1928).Educational Psychology (rev. ed.), Macmillan, New York.Google Scholar
  265. Starch, D. (1930).Experiments in Educational Psychology (revised by author and Harry F. Latshaw), Macmillan, New York.Google Scholar
  266. Stark, J. S., Lowther, M. A., and Hagerty, B. M. K. (1986). Faculty roles and role preferences in ten fields of professional study.Research in Higher Education, 25: 3–30.Google Scholar
  267. Sternberg, R. J. (1981). Testing and cognitive psychology.American Psychologist, 34: 1181–1189.Google Scholar
  268. Storm, C., and Storm, T. (1987). A taxonomic study of the vocabulary of emotions.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53: 805–816.Google Scholar
  269. Strømnes, F. J., and Iivonen, L. (1985). The teaching of the syntax of written language to deaf children knowing no syntax.Human Learning, 4: 251–265.Google Scholar
  270. Suinn, R. M. (1983). Imagery and sports. In Sheikh, A. A. (ed.),Imagery: Current Theory, Research, and Applications, Wiley, New York, pp. 507–534.Google Scholar
  271. Sullivan, A. M., and Skanes, G. R. (1974). Validity of student evaluation of teaching and the characteristics of successful instructors.Journal of Educational Psychology, 66: 584–590.Google Scholar
  272. Terrasi, S., and Airasian, P. W. (1989). The relationship between adaptive behavior and intelligence for special needs students.Psychology in the Schools, 26: 202–208.Google Scholar
  273. Thomassen, A. J. W. M., and Teulings, H. H. M. (1983). The development of handwriting. In Martlew, M. (ed.),The Psychology of Written Language: Developmental and Educational Perspectives, Wiley, New York, pp. 179–213.Google Scholar
  274. Toglia, M. P., and Battig, W. F. (1978).Handbook of Semantic Word Norms, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  275. Troyer, M. B. (1986). A synthesis of research on the characteristics of teacher educators.Journal of Teacher Education, 37: 6–11.Google Scholar
  276. Underwood, B. J., and Richardson, J. (1956). Verbal concept learning as a function of instructions and dominance level.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51: 229–238.Google Scholar
  277. Vaid, J. (1988b). Bilingual memory representation: A further test of dual coding theory.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 42: 84–90.Google Scholar
  278. van der Wissel, A. (1988). Hampered production of words as a characteristic of school failure.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21: 517–518.Google Scholar
  279. van Dijk, T. A., and Kintsch, W. (1983).Strategies of Discourse Comprehension, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  280. Vollmer, F. (1984). Expectancy and academic achievement.Motivation and Emotion, 8: 67–76.Google Scholar
  281. Weaver, W. T. (1983).America's Teacher Quality Problem: Alternatives for Reform, Praeger, New York.Google Scholar
  282. Weinberg, R. S. (1982). The relationship between mental preparation strategies and motor performance: A review and critique.Quest, 33: 195–213.Google Scholar
  283. Weinstein, C. E., Underwood, V. L., Wicker, F. W., and Cubberly, W. E. (1979). Cognitive learning strategies: Verbal and imaginal elaboration. In O'Neill, H. F., and Spielberger, C. D. (eds.),Cognitive and Affective Learning Strategies, Academic Press, New York, pp. 45–75.Google Scholar
  284. Weschler, D. (1974).Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children — Revised, The Psychological Corporation, New York.Google Scholar
  285. Westerback, M. E., and Primavera, L. H. (1988). Anxiety about science and science teaching. In Spielberger, C. D., and Butcher, J. N. (eds.), Advances in Personality Assessment, Vol. 7, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp. 175–202.Google Scholar
  286. Westkott, M. (1979). Feminist criticism of the social sciences. Harvard Educational Review, 49: 422–430.Google Scholar
  287. Wharton, W. P. (1980). Higher-imagery words and the readability of college history texts.Journal of Mental Imagery, 4: 129–147.Google Scholar
  288. Wharton, W. P. (1985). Imagery and the readability of textbooks. In Sheikh, A. A., and Sheikh, K. S. (eds.),Imagery in Education, Baywood, Farmingdale, New York, pp. 145–160.Google Scholar
  289. Williams, J. P., Taylor, M. B., and Ganger, S. (1981). Text variations at the level of the individual sentence and the comprehension of simple expository paragraphs.Journal of Educational Psychology, 73: 851–865.Google Scholar
  290. Winne, P. W., and Marx, R. W. (1977). Reconceptualizing research on teaching.Journal of Educational Psychology, 69: 668–678.Google Scholar
  291. Wittrock, M. C., and Alesandrini, K. (1990). Generation of summaries and analogies and analytic and holistic abilities.American Educational Research Journal, 27: 489–502.Google Scholar
  292. Woolfolk, A. E. (1987).Educational Psychology (3d ed.), Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  293. Wynne, R. D., Gerjuoy, H., and Schiffman, H. (1965). Association test antonym-response set.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4: 354–359.Google Scholar
  294. Yee, A. H. (1969). Do cooperating teachers influence the attitudes of student teachers?Journal of Educational Psychology, 60: 327–332.Google Scholar
  295. Yovetich, W. S., and Paivio, A. (1981). Cognitive processing of Bliss-like symbols by normal populations: A report on four studies.Report of EASE 80 Communication and Handicap, The Finnish Association for Special Education.Google Scholar
  296. Yuille, J. C., and Paivio, A. (1969). Abstractness and the recall of connected discourse.Journal of Educational Psychology, 82: 467–471.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • James M. Clark
    • 1
  • Allan Paivio
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of WinnipegWinnipegCanada
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Western OntarioLondonCanada

Personalised recommendations