Cognitive Therapy and Research

, Volume 17, Issue 1, pp 71–82

The role of automatic thoughts and dysfunctional attitudes in the development and maintenance of experimentally induced dysphoric mood

  • Tore C. Stiles
  • Per Schröder
  • Terje Johansen
Article

Abstract

Sixty-three nondepressed college students were first randomly exposed to either a depression or neutral induction procedure and then randomly to an elation or neutral distraction procedure. It was found that both higher levels of ATQ-30 Factor II thoughts and dysfunctional attitudes predicted postinduction DACL scores, but this effect was not specific for the depression induction condition. Furthermore, it was found that neutral distracting statements were as effective as elation statements in alleviating dysphoric mood, but not for those exhibiting higher levels of ATQ-30 Factor II thoughts.

Key words

automatic thoughts dysfunctional attitudes mood induction distraction 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87 49–74.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Beck, A. T. (1976).Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  3. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979).Cognitive therapy of depression: A treatment manual. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  4. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock., J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression.Archives of General Psychiatry, 5 462–467.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory.American Psychologist, 36 129–148.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Buchwald, A. M., Strack, S., & Coyne, J. C. (1981). Demand characteristics and the Velten mood induction procedure.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49 478–479.Google Scholar
  7. Bumberry, W., Oliver, J. M., & McClure, J. N. (1978). Validation of the Beck Depression Inventory in a university population using psychiatric estimate as the criterion.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46 150–155.Google Scholar
  8. Clark, D.M. (1985). On the induction of depressed mood in the laboratory: Evaluation and comparison of the Velten and musical procedures.Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5 27–50.Google Scholar
  9. Derry, P. A., & Kuiper, N. A. (1981). Schematic processing and self-reference in clinical depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90 286–297.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Dobson, K. S., & Breiter, H. J. (1983). Cognitive assessment of depression: Reliability and validity of three measures.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92 107–109.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Fennell, M. J. V., & Teasdale, J. D. (1984). Effects of distraction of thinking and affect in depressed patients.British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23 65–66.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Goodwin, A. M., & Williams, J. M. G. (1982). Mood-induction research: Its implications for clinical depression.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 20 373–382.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Hollon, S. D., & Kendall, P. C. (1980). Cognitive self-statements in depression: Development of an automatic thoughts questionnaire.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4 383–395.Google Scholar
  14. Hollon, S. D., Kendall, P. C., & Lumry, A. (1986). Specificity of depressotypic cognitions in clinical depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95 52–59.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Lubin, B. (1965). Adjective checklists for measurement of depression.Archives of General Psychiatry, 12 57–62.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Lubin, B., Hornstra, R. K., & Love, A. (1974). Course of depressive mood in a psychiatric population upon application for service at 3- and 12-months re-interviews.Psychological Reports, 34 424–426.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Martin, M. (1985). Neuroticism as predisposition toward depression: A cognitive mechanism.Personality and Individual Differences, 6 353–365.Google Scholar
  18. Oliver, J. M., & Baumgart, E. P. (1985). The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale: Psychometric properties and relation to depression in an unselected adult population.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9 161–167.Google Scholar
  19. Polivy, J. (1981). On the induction of emotion in the laboratory: Discrete moods or multiple affect states?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41 803–817.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Polivy, J., & Doyle, C. (1980). Laboratory induction of mood states through the reading of self-referent mood statements: Affective changes or demand characteristics?Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89 286–290.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Pyszczynski, T., Holt, K., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Depression, self-focused attention, and expectancies for positive and negative future life events for self and others.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 994–1001.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Riskind, J. H., Beck, A. T., & Smucker, M. R. (1983).Psychometric properties of the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale in a clinical population. Paper presented at the World Congress on Behavior Therapy, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  23. Riskind, J. H., & Rholes, W. S. (1985). The cognitive model of depression and mood-induction procedures: A reply to Clark (1983).Behavior Research and Therapy, 23 663–666.Google Scholar
  24. Skeie, T. M., Skeie, S., & Stiles, T. C. (1989). The effectiveness of pleasant imagery and distraction task as coping strategies in alleviating experimentally induced dysphoric mood.Scandinavian Journal of Behavior Therapy, 18 3142.Google Scholar
  25. Stiles, T. C., & Götestam, K. G. (1988). The role of cognitive vulnerability factors in the development of depression. In C. Perris, I. M. Blackburn, & H. Perris (Eds.),Cognitive psychotherapy: Theory and practice (pp. 120–139). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  26. Stiles, T. C., & Götestam, K. G. (1989). The role of automatic negative thoughts in the development of dysphoric mood: An analogue experiment.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 13 161–170.Google Scholar
  27. Stryker, J. P., & McNally, R. J. (1991). Experimental induction of anxious and depressed moods: Are Velten and musical procedures necessary?Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15 33–45.Google Scholar
  28. Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression.Cognition and Emotion, 2 247–274.Google Scholar
  29. Teasdale, J. D., & Rezin, V. (1978). The effects of reducing frequency of negative thoughts on the mood of depressed patients—test of a cognitive model of depression.British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 17 65–74.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for inductions of mood states.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 6 473–482.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978).Development and validation of the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavioral Therapy, Chicago.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Tore C. Stiles
    • 1
  • Per Schröder
    • 1
  • Terje Johansen
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural MedicineUniversity of TrondheimTrondheimNorway

Personalised recommendations