Advertisement

Behavior Genetics

, Volume 11, Issue 5, pp 545–553 | Cite as

Behavioral comparability of wild and domesticated rats

  • Robert Boice
Evolution of Behavior

Abstract

The oft-repeated concern for the lack of behavioral comparability of domestic rats with wild forms ofRattus norvegicus is unfounded. Laboratory rats appear to show the potential for all wild-type behaviors, including the most dramatic social postures. Moreover, domestics are capable of assuming a feral existence without difficulty, one where they readily behave in a fashion indistinguishable from wild rats. The one behavioral difference that is clearly established concerns performance in laboratory learning paradigms. The superiority of domestics in these laboratory tasks speaks more to quieting the concerns of degeneracy theorists than to problems of using domestic Norway rats as subjects representative of their species.

Key Words

Rattus norvegicus wild rats domestication feralization 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bamber, R. T., and Boice, R. (1972). The labyrinth method of comparing wild and domestic rats.Psychonom. Sci.,29:161–163.Google Scholar
  2. Barnett, S. A. (1963).The Rat, Aldine, Chicago.Google Scholar
  3. Boice, R. (1966). The problem of domestication in the laboratory rat, Unpublished dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing.Google Scholar
  4. Boice, R. (1968). Conditioned licking in wild and domestic Norway rats.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 66: 696–799.Google Scholar
  5. Boice, R. (1970). Effect of domestication on avoidance learning in Norway rats.Psychonom. Sci. 18:13–14.Google Scholar
  6. Boice, R. (1971a). Excessive water intake in captive Norway rats with scarmarkings.Physiol. Behav. 7:723–725.Google Scholar
  7. Boice, R. (1971b). Laboratorizing the wild rat (Rattus norvegicus).Behav. Res. Meth. Instru. 3:177–182.Google Scholar
  8. Boice, R. (1972). Some behavioral tests of domestication in Norway rats.Behaviour 42:198–221.Google Scholar
  9. Boice, R. (1973). Domestication.Psychol. Bull. 80:215–230.Google Scholar
  10. Boice, R. (1977). Burrows of wild and domestic rats: Effects of domestication, outdoor raising, age, experience, and maternal state.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 91:649–661.Google Scholar
  11. Boice, R. (1981a). Captivity and feralization.Psychol. Bull. 89:407–421.Google Scholar
  12. Boice, R. (1981b).Ethological Psychology, Brooks/Cole, Monterey (in press).Google Scholar
  13. Boice, R., Denny, M. R., and Evans, T. A. (1967). A comparison of albino and wild rats in shuttlebox avoidance.Psychonom. Sci. 8:271–272.Google Scholar
  14. Boice, R., Hughes, C. W., and Bamber, R. T. (1976). Learning comparisons of wild and domestic Norway rats. Unpublished paper.Google Scholar
  15. Clark, M., and Galef, B. G. (1980). Effects of rearing environment on adrenal weights, sexual development, and behavior in Gerbils: An examination of Richter's domestication hypothesis.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 94:857–863.Google Scholar
  16. Connor, J. L. (1972). Genetic mechanisms responsible for the domestication of the laboratory mouse. Paper presented at the meeting of the Behavior Genetics Association.Google Scholar
  17. Daly, M. (1973). Early stimulation of rodents: A critical review of present interpretation.Br. J. Psychol. 64:435–460.Google Scholar
  18. Darwin, C. (1958).Origin of Species, John Murray, London.Google Scholar
  19. Darwin, C. (1868).The Variation of Plants and Animals Under Domestication, John Murray, London.Google Scholar
  20. Epstein, H. (1971).The Origins of the Domestic Animals of Africa, Vol. I, Africana, New York.Google Scholar
  21. Hale, E. B. (1969). Domestication and the evolution of behavior. In Hafez, E. S. E. (ed.),The Behavior of Domestic Animals, 2nd ed., Balliere, Tindall and Cassell, London.Google Scholar
  22. Hughes, C. W. (1975). Early experience in domestication.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 88:407–417.Google Scholar
  23. Hughes, C. W., and Boice, R. (1973). Domestication, sophistication, and avoidance in norway rats.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 84:408–413.Google Scholar
  24. Hughes, C. W., Settle, A. V., and Boice, R. (1976). Four indices of domestication in Norway rats.Bull. Psychonom. Soc. 8:171–174.Google Scholar
  25. Kavanau, J. L. (1964). Behavior: Confinement, adaptation, and compulsory regimes in laboratory studies.Science 143:490.Google Scholar
  26. King, H. D. (1939). Life processes in gray Norway rats during fourteen years in captivity.Am. Anat. Mem. 17:1–72.Google Scholar
  27. Lockard, R. B. (1968). The albino rat: A defensible choice or a bad habit?Am. Psychol. 23:734–742.Google Scholar
  28. Logan, F. A., and Boice, R. (1969). Aggressive behaviors of paired rodents in an avoidance context.Behaviour 34:161–183.Google Scholar
  29. Lorenz, K. (1940). Durch Domestikation verursachte Storungen arteigenen Verhaltens.Z. Psychol. Charakt. 59:2–82.Google Scholar
  30. Millar, R. D. (1975). Free-operant comparisons of wild and domestic Norway rats.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 89:913–922.Google Scholar
  31. Miller, D. B. (1977). Social displays of Mallard Ducks (Anas platyrhynchos): Effects of domestication.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 91:221–232.Google Scholar
  32. Price, E. O. (1976). The laboratory animal and its environment. In McSheeny, T. (ed.),Control of the Animal House Environment, Laboratory Animals, London.Google Scholar
  33. Price, E. O., and King, J. A. (1968). Domestication and adaptation. In Hafez, E. S. E. (ed.),Adaptation of Domestic Animals, Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
  34. Ratner, S. C., and Boice, R. (1975). Effects of domestication on behavior. In Hafez, E. S. E. (ed.),Behaviour of Domestic Animals, 3rd ed., Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore.Google Scholar
  35. Richter, C. P. (1954). The effects of domestication and selection on the behavior of the Norway rat.J. Natl. Cancer Inst. 15:727–738.Google Scholar
  36. Richter, C. P. (1959). Rats, man and the welfare state.Am. Psychol. 14:18–28.Google Scholar
  37. Richter, C. P., and Mosier, H. D. (1954). Maximum sodium chloride intake and thirst in domesticated and wild Norway rats.Am. J. Physiol. 176:213–222.Google Scholar
  38. Robinson, R. (1965).Genetics of the Norway Rat, Pergamon, Oxford.Google Scholar
  39. Skinner, B. F. (1966). The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior.Science 153:1205–1213.Google Scholar
  40. Stone, C. P. (1932). Wildness and savageness in rats of different strains. In Lashley, K. S. (ed.),Studies in the Dynamics of Behavior, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  41. Thiessen, D. D. (1966). The relation of social position and wounding to exploratory behavior and organ weights in house mice.J. Mammal. 47:28–34.Google Scholar
  42. Tinbergen, N. (1965).Animal Behavior, Time-Life, New York.Google Scholar
  43. Ulrich, R. (1966). Pain as a cause of aggression.Am. Zool. 6:643–662.Google Scholar
  44. Yerkes, R. M. (1913). The heredity of savageness and wildness in rats.J. Anim. Behav. 3:286–296.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1981

Authors and Affiliations

  • Robert Boice
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyState University of New York at AlbanyAlbany

Personalised recommendations