Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 5, Issue 2, pp 143–171 | Cite as

A cognitive model of musical sight-reading

  • Thomas Wolf
Article

Abstract

The author, a professional flutist and psychologist, interviewed four pianists noted for their sight-reading abilities. The results of the interviews are considered from several points of view. Sight-reading is analyzed as a problem in pattern recognition: a movement from a sonata by Handel is used to illustrate the principle of scanning for familiar patterns. The close relationship between musical sight-reading and the reading of conventional texts is also suggested. Finally, drawing on the findings of other studies, a cognitive model of musical sight-reading is proposed. The schematic model of interlocking information-processing systems explains the differences between skilled and unskilled sight-readers; it also explains why some experienced, professional musicians are poor sight-readers. Verification of the model is provided in an additional section in which conversations with unskilled sight-readers are reported.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Atkinson, R. C., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. InThe Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 2, Academic Press, New York, pp. 89–195.Google Scholar
  2. Bean, K. L. (1938). An approach to the reading of music.Psychol. Monogr. 226: 1–80.Google Scholar
  3. Broadbent, D. E. (1958).Perception and Communication, Pergamon Press, New York.Google Scholar
  4. de Groot, A. D. (1965).Thought and Choice in Chess, Mouton, The Hague.Google Scholar
  5. Geyer, J. J., and Kolers, P. A. (1974). Reading as information processing. In Voight, M. J. (ed.),Advances in Librarianship, Academic Press, New York, pp. 175–237.Google Scholar
  6. Gregg, L. W., and Simon, G. A. (1967). An information-processing explanation of one-trial and incremental learning.J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 6: 780–787.Google Scholar
  7. Huey, E. G. (1968).The Psychology and Pedagogy of Reading (1908), MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.Google Scholar
  8. Kolers, P. A. (1970). Three stages of reading. In Levin, H., and Williams, J. P. (eds.),Basic Studies on Reading, Basic Books, New York, pp. 90–118.Google Scholar
  9. Miller, G. A. (1956). The magic number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information.Psychol. Rev. 63: 81–97.Google Scholar
  10. Neisser, U. (1967).Cognitive Psychology, Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.Google Scholar
  11. Simon, H. A., and Barenfeld, M. (1969). Information-processing analysis of perceptual processes in problem solving.Psychol. Rev. 76: 473–483.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1976

Authors and Affiliations

  • Thomas Wolf
    • 1
  1. 1.Graduate School of EducationHarvard UniversityCambridge

Personalised recommendations