Advertisement

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 4, Issue 1, pp 75–87 | Cite as

Hesitation and semantic planning in speech

  • Brian Butterworth
Article

Abstract

Samples of spontaneous speech were analyzed according to their distributions of phonations and silences. Some of these exhibited cyclic, or “rhythmic,” patterns, in the sense defined by Goldman-Eisler. Transcripts of three such samples were subjected to a segmentation procedure carried out by independent judges utilizing a common semantic intuition. Points in the transcripts where agreement was high among the judges were found to correspond with the beginnings of temporal cycles, and agreed semantic segments coincided with sentence or clause boundaries and usually consisted of several clauses and more than one sentence. It is argued that a theory of speech generation must contain provision for semantic integration at the suprasentential level.

Keywords

Cognitive Psychology Temporal Cycle Spontaneous Speech Segmentation Procedure Speech Generation 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Boomer, D. S. (1965). Hesitation and grammatical encoding.Lang. Speech 8: 148–158.Google Scholar
  2. Boomer, D. S., and Laver, J. D. M. (1968). Slips of the tongue.Brit. J. Disorders Commun. 3: 2–11.Google Scholar
  3. Butterworth, B. L. (1972). Semantic analyses of the phasing of fluency in spontaneous speech. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University College, London.Google Scholar
  4. Butterworth, B. L. (1973). Lexical choice, grammatical options and hesitations in speech. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
  5. Goldman-Eisler, F. (1958). Speech production and the predictability of words in context.Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 10: 96–106.Google Scholar
  6. Goldman-Eisler, F. (1961). Hesitation and information in speech. In Cherry, C. (ed.),Information Theory (Fourth London Symposium, 1960), Butterworth, London.Google Scholar
  7. Goldman-Eisler, F. (1968).Psycholinguistics: Experiments in Spontaneous Speech, Academic Press, London.Google Scholar
  8. Goldman-Eisler, F. (1972). Pauses, clauses, sentences.Lang. Speech 15: 103–113.Google Scholar
  9. Henderson, A., Goldman-Eisler, F., and Skarbek, A. (1966). Sequential temporal patterns in spontaneous speech.Lang. Speech 9: 207–216.Google Scholar
  10. Jackson, H. J. (1878). On affections of speech from disease of the brain. Reprinted inSelected Writings of Hughlings Jackson, Vol. II (1958), Basic Books, New York, pp. 155–170.Google Scholar
  11. Jaffe, J., Breskin, S., and Gerstman, L. J. (1972). Random generation of apparent speech rhythms.Lang. Speech 15: 68–71.Google Scholar
  12. Lay, C. H., and Paivio, A. (1969). The effects of task difficulty and anxiety on hesitation in speech.Canad. J. Behav. Sci. 1: 25–37.Google Scholar
  13. Maclay, H., and Osgood, C. E. (1959). Hesitation phenomena in spontaneous English speech.Word 15: 19–44.Google Scholar
  14. Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., and Pribram, K. H. (1960).Plans and the Structure of Behavior, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.Google Scholar
  15. Reynolds, A., and Paivio, A. (1968). Cognitive and emotional determinants of speech.Canad. J. Psychol. 22:164–175.Google Scholar
  16. Tannenbaum, P. H., Williams, F., and Hillier, C. S. (1965). Word predictability in the environments of hesitations.J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 4: 134–140.Google Scholar
  17. Wundt, W. (1912). Die Sprache. In Blumenthal, A. L. (ed.),Language and Psychology (1970), Wiley, New York, pp. 20–33.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1975

Authors and Affiliations

  • Brian Butterworth
    • 1
  1. 1.Psychological LaboratoryUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeEngland

Personalised recommendations