Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 13, Issue 3, pp 205–234 | Cite as

Diurnal variation in the Positive Affects

  • Lee Anna Clark
  • David Watson
  • Jay Leeka
Article

Abstract

We examined the circadian rhythm of two basic dimensions of mood—Positive Affect (PA) and Negative Affect (NA). Subjects were 196 college students in two studies who completed a mood rating form approximately seven times a day for 1 week. Significant diurnal variation was found to exist in overall Positive Affect, and in four component content areas, but not in Negative Affect. Specifically, all components of PA rose sharply from early morning until noon, remained relatively constant until 9 p.m., and then fell rapidly. The rise and fall of PA were quite robust across subjects, but the apparently static level of PA from noon to 9 p.m. was shown to result from averaging across marked individual differences in the time of peak PA. Various potential sources of these individual differences—“morningness-eveningness,” introversion-extraversion, and neuroticism/dysphoria—were investigated. Neither of the personality variables accounted for the significant variation in diurnal mood patterning, but clearly defined morning and evening types did show different patterns. Possible links between the mood pattern obtained and biologically based circadian rhythms, which have been postulated to be dysregulated in depression and mania, are discussed.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aschoff, J., Giedke, H., Poppel, E., & Wever, R. (1972). The influence of sleep-interruption and of sleep-deprivation on circadian rhythms in human performance. In W. P. Colquhuon (Ed.),Aspects of human efficiency: Diurnal rhythm and loss of sleep (pp. 135–150). London: The English Universities Press.Google Scholar
  2. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation.Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100.Google Scholar
  3. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression.Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.Google Scholar
  4. Blake, M. J. F. (1967). Relationship between circadian rhythm of body temperature and introversion-extraversion.Nature, 215, 896–897.Google Scholar
  5. Blake, M. J. F., & Corcoran, D. W. J. (1972). Introversion-extraversion and circadian rhythms. In W. P. Colquhuon (Ed.),Aspects of human efficiency: Diurnal rhythm and loss of sleep (pp. 261–272). London: The English Universities Press.Google Scholar
  6. Christie, M., & Venables, P. (1973). Mood changes in relation to age, EPI scores, time and day.British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12, 61–72.Google Scholar
  7. Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1988). Mood and the mundane: Relations between daily life events and self-reported mood.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 296–308.Google Scholar
  8. Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1990).Predicting momentary mood from ordinary life events and personality. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  9. Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (in press). Theoretical and empirical issues in differentiating depression from anxiety. In J. Becker & A. Kleinman (Eds.),Psychosocial aspects of depression (pp. 39–65). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Clements, P. R., Hafer, M. D., & Vermillion, M. E. (1976). Psychometric, diurnal, and electrophysical correlates of activation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 387–394.Google Scholar
  11. Coleman, R. M. (1986).Wide awake at 3 a.m.: By choice or by chance? New York: W. H. Freeman & Co.Google Scholar
  12. Colquhuon, W. P. (1960). Temperament, inspection efficiency, and time of day.Ergonomics, 3, 377–378.Google Scholar
  13. Colquhuon, W. P., & Corcoran, D. W. J. (1964). The effects of time of day and social isolation on the relationship between temperature and performance.British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 3, 226–231.Google Scholar
  14. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668–678.Google Scholar
  15. Curtis, G. C., Fogel, M. L., McEvoy, D., & Zarate, C. (1966). The effect of sustained affect on the diurnal rhythm of adrenal cortical activity.Psychosomatic Medicine, 28, 696–713.Google Scholar
  16. Depue, R. A. (1979).The psychobiology of the depressive disorders: Implications for the effects of stress. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  17. Depue, R. A, Krauss, S. P., & Spoont, M. R. (1987). A two-dimensional threshold model of seasonal bipolar affective disorder. In D. Magnusson & A. Ohrman (Eds.),Psychopathology: An interactional perspective (pp. 95–123). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  18. Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1105–1117.Google Scholar
  19. Diener, E., Larsen, R. J., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). Person × situation interactions: Choice of situations and congruence response models.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 580–592.Google Scholar
  20. Emmons, R. A., & Diener, E. (1985). Personality correlates of subjective well-being.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 89–97.Google Scholar
  21. Eysenck, H., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1968).Manual for the Eysenck Personality Inventory. San Diego: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.Google Scholar
  22. Folkard, S. (1975). Diurnal variation in logical reasoning.British Journal of Psychology, 66, 1–8.Google Scholar
  23. Froberg, J. (1977). Twenty-four hour patterns in human performance, subjective and physiological variables and differences between morning and evening active subjects.Biological Psychology, 5, 119–134.Google Scholar
  24. Froberg, J., Karlsson, C. G., Levi, L., & Lidberg, L. (1972). Circadian variations in performance, psychological ratings, catecholamine excretion, and diuresis during prolonged sleep deprivation.International Journal of Psychobiology, 2, 23–36.Google Scholar
  25. Froberg, J., Karlsson, C. G., Levi, L., & Lidberg, L. (1975). Circadian rhythms of catecholamine excretion, shooting range performance and self-ratings of fatigue during sleep deprivation.Biological Psychology, 2, 175–188.Google Scholar
  26. Hall, P., Spear, F. G., & Stirland, D. (1964). Diurnal variation of subjective mood in depressive states.Psychiatric Quarterly, 38, 529–536.Google Scholar
  27. Healy, D., & Williams, J. M. G. (1988). Dysrhythmia, dysphoria, and depression: The interaction of learned helplessness and circadian dysrhythmia in the pathogenesis of depression.Psychological Bulletin, 103, 163–178.Google Scholar
  28. Hepburn, C., Ortiz, V., & Locksley, A. (1984). Morning and evening people: Examination of an identifying scale and of social- and self-perceptions of personality differences.Journal of Research in Personality, 18, 99–109.Google Scholar
  29. Horne, J. A., & Ostberg, O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms.International Journal of Chronobiology, 4, 97–110.Google Scholar
  30. Horne, J. A., & Ostberg, O. (1977). Individual differences in Human circadian rhythms.Biological Psychology, 5, 179–190.Google Scholar
  31. Isen, A. M., Johnson, M. M. S., Mertz, E., & Robinson, G. F. (1985). The influence of positive affect on the unusualness of word associations.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1413–1426.Google Scholar
  32. Kagan, J. (1988). The meanings of personality predicates.American Psychologist, 43, 614–620.Google Scholar
  33. Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., & Ingram, R. E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding use of the Beck Depression Inventory.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289–299.Google Scholar
  34. Kendall, P. C., & Watson, D. (1989).Anxiety and depression: Distinctive and overlapping features. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  35. Kiloh, L. G., Andrews, G., Neilson, M., & Bianchi, G. N. (1972). The relationship of the syndromes called endogenous and neurotic depression.British Journal of Psychiatry, 121, 183–196.Google Scholar
  36. Kleitman, N. (1963).Sleep and wakefulness (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  37. Larsen, R. J. (1985). Individual differences in circadian activity rhythm and personality.Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 305–311.Google Scholar
  38. Lavie, P. (1986). Ultrashort sleep-waking schedules III: “Gates and the forbidden zones for sleep”.EEG and Clinical Neurophysiology, 63, 414–425.Google Scholar
  39. Lawton, M. P. (1984). The varieties of well-being. In C. Z. Malatesta & C. E. Izard (Eds.),Emotion in adult development (pp. 67–84). Beverly Hills: Sage Publication.Google Scholar
  40. Levy, D. A. (1985). Optimism and pessimism: Relationships to circadian rhythms.Psychological Reports, 57, 1123–1126.Google Scholar
  41. Mellerup, E. T., & Rafaelsen, O. J. (1979). Circadian rhythms in manic-melancholic disorders. In W. B. Essman & L. Walzelli (Eds.),Current developments in psychopharmacology (Vol. 5, pp. 51–66). New York: Spectrum.Google Scholar
  42. Monk, T. H., Leng, V. C., Folkard, S., & Weitzman, E. D. (1983). Circadian rhythms in subjective alertness and core body temperature.Chronobiologica, 10, 49–55.Google Scholar
  43. Moore-Ede, M. C., Czeisler, C. A., & Richardson, G. S. (1983a). Circadian timekeeping in health and disease.New England Journal of Medicine, 309, 469–476.Google Scholar
  44. Moore-Ede, M. C., Czeisler, C. A., & Richardson, G. S. (1983b). Circadian timekeeping in health and disease. Part 2. Clinical implications of circadian rhythmicity.New England Journal of Medicine, 309, 530–536.Google Scholar
  45. Mura, E. L., & Levy, D. A. (1986). Relationship between neuroticism and circadian rhythm.Psychological Reports, 58, 298.Google Scholar
  46. Nelson, J. C., & Charney, D. S. (1981). The symptoms of major depressive illness.American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 1–13.Google Scholar
  47. Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we know: Verbal reports on mental processes.Psychological Review, 84, 231–279.Google Scholar
  48. Oliver, J. M., & Burkham, R. (1979). Depression in university students: Duration, relation to calendar time, prevalence and demographic correlates.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 667–670.Google Scholar
  49. Ostberg, O. (1973). Circadian rhythms of food intake and oral temperature in ‘Morning’ and ‘Evening’ groups of individuals.Ergonomics, 16, 203–209.Google Scholar
  50. Patkai, P. (1971). Interindividual differences in diurnal variations in alertness, performance, and adrenaline excretion.Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 81 35–46.Google Scholar
  51. Pennebaker, J. W. (1982).The psychology of physical symptoms. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  52. Phillips, D. L. (1967). Social participation and happiness.American Journal of Sociology, 72, 479–488.Google Scholar
  53. Pilowsky, I., Levine, S., & Boulton, D. M. (1969). The classification of depression by numerical taxonomy.British Journal of Psychiatry, 115, 937–945.Google Scholar
  54. Rafaelsen, O. J., & Mellerup, E. T. (1977). Circadian rhythms in depressive disorders. In S. Garatini (Ed.),Symposia medica hoecht (Vol. 13, pp. 410–417). Stuttgart: Schattaner-Verlag.Google Scholar
  55. Robbins, P. R., & Tanck, R. H. (1987). A study of diurnal patterns of depressed mood.Motivation and Emotion, 11, 37–49.Google Scholar
  56. Rossi, A. S., & Rossi, P. E. (1977). Body time and social time: Mood patterns by menstrual cycle phase and day of the week.Social Science Research, 6, 273–308.Google Scholar
  57. Smith, T. W. (1979). Happiness: Time trends, seasonal variations, intersurvey differences, and other mysteries.Social Psychology Quarterly, 42, 18–30.Google Scholar
  58. SAS Institute Inc. (1982).SAS User's Guide: Statistics, 1982 Edition. Cary, NC: Author.Google Scholar
  59. Stallone, F., Huba, G. J., Lawlor, W. G., & Fieve, R. R. (1973). Longitudinal studies of diurnal variations in depression: A sample of 643 patient days.British Journal of Psychiatry, 123, 311–318.Google Scholar
  60. Stone, A. A., Hedges, S. M., Neale, J. M., & Satin, M. S. (1985). Prospective and cross-sectional mood reports offer no evidence of a “Blue Monday” phenomenon.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 129–134.Google Scholar
  61. Stroebel, C. F. (1985). Biological rhythms in psychiatry. In H. I. Kaplan & B. J. Sadock (Eds.),Comprehensive textbook in psychiatry (4th ed., pp. 67–70). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
  62. Taub, J. M., & Berger, R. J. (1974). Diurnal variations in mood as asserted by self-report and verbal content analysis.Journal of Psychiatry Research, 10, 83–88.Google Scholar
  63. Tellegen, A. (1982).Brief manual for the Differential Personality Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis.Google Scholar
  64. Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety with an emphasis on self-report. In A. H. Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.),Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 681–706). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  65. Tellegen, A. (in press).Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
  66. Thayer, R. E. (1967). Measurement of activation through self-report.Psychological Reports, 20, 663–678.Google Scholar
  67. Thayer, R. E. (1978). Toward a psychological theory of multidimensional activation (arousal).Motivation and Emotion, 2, 1–34.Google Scholar
  68. Thayer, R. E. (1986). Factor analytic and reliability studies on the Activation-Deactivation Adjective Check List.Psychological Reports, 42, 747–756.Google Scholar
  69. Thayer, R. E. (1987). Problem perception, optimism, and related states as a function of time of day (diurnal rhythm) and moderate exercise: Two arousal systems in interaction.Motivation and Emotion, 11, 19–36.Google Scholar
  70. Thayer, R. E. (1989).The biopsychology of mood and arousal. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  71. Thayer, R. E., Takahashi, P. J., & Pauli, J. A. (1988). Multidimensional arousal states, diurnal rhythms, cognitive and social processes, and extraversion.Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 15–24.Google Scholar
  72. Watson, D. (1988a). Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of Positive and Negative Affect: Their relation to health complaints, perceived stress, and daily activities.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1020–1030.Google Scholar
  73. Watson, D. (1988b). The vicissitudes of mood measurement: Effects of varying descriptors, time frames, and response formats on measures of Positive and Negative Affect.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 128–141.Google Scholar
  74. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative Affectivity: The disposition to experience aversive emotional states.Psychological Bulletin, 96, 465–490.Google Scholar
  75. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (in press). Extraversion and its positive emotional core. In S. Briggs, R. Hogan, & W. Jones (Eds.),Handbook of personality psychology. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  76. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1990).Affects separable and inseparable: A hierarchical model of emotional experience. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  77. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988). Positive and Negative Affect and their relation to anxiety and depressive disorders.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 346–353.Google Scholar
  78. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.Google Scholar
  79. Watson, D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the central role of Negative Affectivity.Psychological Review, 96, 234–254.Google Scholar
  80. Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood.Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235.Google Scholar
  81. Watts, C., Cox, T., & Robson, J. (1983). Morningness-eveningness and diurnal variations in self-reported mood.Journal of Psychology, 113, 251–256.Google Scholar
  82. Webb, W. B., & Bonnet, M. H. (1978). The sleep of ‘morning’ and ‘evening’ types.Biological Psychology, 7, 29–35.Google Scholar
  83. Wehr, T. A., & Wirz-Justice, A. (1982). Circadian rhythm mechanisms in affective illness and in antidepressant drug action.Pharmacopsychiatria, 15, 31–39.Google Scholar
  84. Williams, J. G., Barlow, D. H., & Agras, W. S. (1975). Diurnal variation in depression—Is it there?Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 161, 59–62.Google Scholar
  85. Zevon, M. A., & Tellegen, A. (1982). The structure of mood change: An idiographic/nomothetic analysis.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 111–122.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1989

Authors and Affiliations

  • Lee Anna Clark
    • 1
  • David Watson
    • 1
  • Jay Leeka
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologySouthern Methodist UniversityDallas
  2. 2.Veteran's AdministrationDallas

Personalised recommendations