Advertisement

Political Behavior

, Volume 14, Issue 1, pp 67–87 | Cite as

Bases of political judgments: The role of stereotypic and nonstereotypic information

  • Ellen D. Riggle
  • Victor C. Ottati
  • Robert S. WyerJr.
  • James Kuklinski
  • Norbert Schwarz
Article

Abstract

Three experiments investigated the role of stereotypic and nonstereotypic criteria in judgments of political candidates. The effects of physical attractiveness, political party, and stands on specific issues on both absolute and comparative judgments of political candidates were examined to evaluate three hypotheses about stereotype and attribute use. In the absence of other information, candidates' physical attractiveness (conveyed through photographs) had a substantial influence on subjects' global evaluations of them and inferences of both their personal qualities and their political ideology. When other information about the candidates' party membership and stands on specific issues was available, however, the candidates' attractiveness had no effect on the evaluations of them. When subjects made judgments of only one candidate, subjects relied exclusively on the candidate's voting record. When subjects were asked to make comparative judgments of two candidates, however, they based their judgments on each candidate's party membership and not on their respective voting records. Implications of these results for the processes that underlie political judgments and decisions are evaluated.

Keywords

Political Party Global Evaluation Substantial Influence Political Ideology Physical Attractiveness 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Asher, H. B. (1983). Voting behavior research in the 1980s: An examination of some old and new problem areas. In A. W. Finifter (ed.),Political Science: The State of the Discipline. Washington, DC: American Political Science Association.Google Scholar
  2. Bodenhausen, G. V., and M. Lichtenstein (1987). Social stereotypes and information-processing strategies: The impact of task complexity.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52:871–880.Google Scholar
  3. Bodenhausen, G. V., and R. S. Wyer (1985). Effects of stereotypes on decision-making and information-processing strategies.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48:267–282.Google Scholar
  4. Campbell, A., P. E. Converse, W. E. Miller, and D. E. Stokes (1960).The American Voter. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  5. Campbell, A., G. Gurin, and W. E. Miller (1954).The Voter Decides. Evanston, IL: Row and Peterson.Google Scholar
  6. Carlston, D. E. (1980). The recall and use of observed behavioral episodes and inferred traits of social inference processes.Journal of Social Experimental Psychology 16:779–804.Google Scholar
  7. Conover, P., and S. Feldman (1986). The role of inference in the perception of political candidates. In R. R. Lau and D. O. Sears (eds.),Political Cognition: The 19th Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  8. Deaux, K., and L. L. Lewis (1984). Structure of gender stereotypes: Interrelationships among components and gender label.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46:991–1104.Google Scholar
  9. Dion, K. K., E. Bersheid, and E. H. Walster (1972). What is beautiful is good.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 24:285–290.Google Scholar
  10. Futoran, G. C., and R. S. Wyer, Jr. (1986). The effects of traits and gender-stereotypes on occupational suitability judgments and the recall of judgment-relevant information.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 22:475–503.Google Scholar
  11. Hamilton, David L., ed. (1981).Cognitive Processes in Stereotyping and Intergroup Behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  12. Herstein, J. (1981). Keeping the voter's limits in mind: A cognitive process analysis of decision making in voting.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 40:843–861.Google Scholar
  13. Hinkley, B. (1981).Congressional Elections. Washington, DC: Congressional Quarterly Press.Google Scholar
  14. Hinkley, B., R. Hofstetter, and J. Kessel (1974). Information and the vote: A comparative election study.American Politics Quarterly 2:131–158.Google Scholar
  15. Hong, S., and R. S. Wyer (1989). Country of origin, attributes, and product evaluations: The effects of time delay between information and judgments.Journal of Consumer Research 16:175–187.Google Scholar
  16. Kinder, D. R. (1986). Presidential character revisited. In R. R. Lau and D. O. Sears (eds.),Political Cognition (pp. 233–255). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  17. Kinder, D. R., and R. P. Abelson (1981). Appraising presidential candidates: Personality and affect in the 1980 campaign. Paper delivered at the 1981 annual meeting of the American Political Science Association, New York, 3–6 September.Google Scholar
  18. Knight, K. (1985). Ideology in the 1980 election: Ideological sophistication does matter.Journal of Politics 47:828–853.Google Scholar
  19. Locksley, A., E. Borgida, N. Brekke, and C. Hepburn (1980). Sex stereotypes and social judgment.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39:821–831.Google Scholar
  20. Locksley, A., C. Hepburn, and V. Ortiz (1982). Social stereotypes and judgments of individuals: An instance of the base-rate fallacy.Journal of Experimental Psychology 18:12–42.Google Scholar
  21. Luskin, R. C. (1987). Measuring political sophistication.American Journal of Political Science 31:856–899.Google Scholar
  22. Markus, G. B., and P. E. Converse (1979). A dynamic simultaneous equation model of electoral choice.American Political Science Review 73:1055–1070.Google Scholar
  23. Miller, A. H., M. P. Wattenburg, and O. Malanchuk (1986). Schematic assessments of political candidates.American Political Science Review 80:521–540.Google Scholar
  24. Montgomery, H. (1983). Decision rules and the search for a dominance structure: Towards process model of decision making. In P. Humphreys, O. Svenson, and A. Vari (eds.),Analyzing and Aiding Decision Processes (pp. 343–369). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
  25. Ottati, V. C. (1990). Determinants of political judgments: The joint influence of normative and heuristic rules of inference.Political Behavior 12:159–179.Google Scholar
  26. Ottati, V. C., E. D. Riggle, R. S. Wyer, N. Schwarz, and J. Kuklinski (1989). Cognitive and affective bases of opinion survey responses.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57:404–415.Google Scholar
  27. Ottati, V. C., and R. S. Wyer (1990). The cognitive mediators of political information processing. In J. A. Ferejohn and J. H. Kuklinski (eds.),Information and Democratic Process. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  28. Page, B. I., and R. A. Brody (1972). Policy voting and the electoral process.American Political Science Review 66:979–995.Google Scholar
  29. Rabinowitz, G. (1978). On the nature of political issues.American Journal of Political Science 22:793–817.Google Scholar
  30. Rahn, W. M. (1989). The role of partisan stereotypes in information processing about political candidates. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Political Science Association, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
  31. Repass, D. E. (1971). Issue salience and party choice.American Political Science Review 65:389–400.Google Scholar
  32. Riggle, E. D., T. L. Budesheim, and R. S. Wyer (1990). Temporal effects on judgments of political candidates. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Political Science Association, San Francisco, CA.Google Scholar
  33. Rosenberg, S. W., L. Bohan, P. McCafferty, and K. Harris (1986). The image and the vote: The effect of candidate presentation of voter preference.American Journal of Political Science 30:108–127.Google Scholar
  34. Rosenberg, S. W., and P. McCafferty (1987). The image and the vote: Manipulating voters' preferences.Public Opinion Quarterly 51:31–47.Google Scholar
  35. Schwarz, N., and R. S. Wyer (1985). Effects of rank ordering stimuli on magnitude ratings of these and other stimuli.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 21:30–46.Google Scholar
  36. Sherman, S. J., K. Ahlm, L. Berman, and S. Lynn (1978). Contrast effects and their relationship to subsequent behavior.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 14:340–350.Google Scholar
  37. Sigelman, C. K., D. B. Thomas, L. Sigelman, and F. D. Ribich (1986). Gender, physical attractiveness, and electability: An experimental investigation of voter biases.Journal of Applied Social Psychology 16:229–248.Google Scholar
  38. Sniderman, P. M., J. M. Glaser, and R. Griffin (1990). Information and electoral choice. In J. A. Ferejohn and J. H. Kuklinski (eds.),Information and Democratic Processes (pp. 117–135). Urbana, IL: University of Illinois.Google Scholar
  39. Weisberg, H. F., and J. G. Rusk (1970). Dimensions of candidate evaluations.American Political Science Review 60:19–28.Google Scholar
  40. Wyer, R. S., and T. K. Srull (1989).Human Cognition in Its Social Context. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ellen D. Riggle
    • 1
  • Victor C. Ottati
    • 2
  • Robert S. WyerJr.
    • 3
  • James Kuklinski
    • 3
  • Norbert Schwarz
    • 4
  1. 1.University of KentuckyLexington
  2. 2.State University of New YorkStony Brook
  3. 3.University of IllinoisUrbana-Champaign
  4. 4.ZUMAMannheimFederal Republic of Germany

Personalised recommendations