Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 2, Issue 2, pp 97–175 | Cite as

Curiosity and learning

  • Daniel E. Berlyne
Article

Abstract

This posthumous fragment of a book that Dr. Berlyne was writing at the time of his death was sent to MOTIVATION AND EMOTION by his colleagues at the University of Toronto. Its appearance in print is by permission of the Berlyne family and with appreciation to F. G. Hare and John Ogilvie of the University of Toronto and Seymour Weingarten of Plenum Publishing corporation, who were instrumental in providing a copy of the manuscript to the editor; to George Rappolt and Pat Monahan, of Clark University, who assisted in compiling the bibliography; and to Dr. Edward L. Walker, who provided some editing and wrote the brief introduction.

References to figures (in Chapters 1 and 3) were deleted, as no figures could be found to accompany the manuscript. In any case, it appeared that these would have merely supplemented the text and were neither new nor original, insofar as could be judged. The reader may further note that perhaps a half dozen references are missing, and others may not have been those intended by Dr. Berlyne.

While it seems unlikely that the chapters presented here were either complete or in their final draft stage, it was nevertheless felt that publication of even this fragment of Daniel Berlyne's last major work, with only minimal editing, would be of value to fellow students of motivation theory.

M.H.A.

Keywords

Social Psychology Motivation Theory Fellow Student Major Work Final Draft 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adametz, J. H. Rate of recovery of functioning in cats with rostral reticular lesions.Journal of Neurosurgery 1959,16 85–98.Google Scholar
  2. Allport, G. W., & Vernon, P. E.A study of values. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1931.Google Scholar
  3. Ashida, S. The effects of deprivation and post-deprivation on the heart rate of rats.Psychonomic Science 1969,14 123–124.Google Scholar
  4. Attneave, F. Physical determinants of the judged complexity of shapes.Journal of Experimental Psychology 1957,53 221–227.Google Scholar
  5. Bartenwerfer, H. Über Art und Bedeutung der Beziehung zwischen Pulsfrequenz und skalierter psychischer Anspannung.Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie 1963,10 455–470.Google Scholar
  6. Beebe-Center, J. G.The psychology of pleasantness and unpleasantness. Princeton: Van Nostrand, 1932.Google Scholar
  7. Bélanger, D., & Feldman, S. M. Effects of water deprivation upon heart rate and instrumental activity in the rat.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1962,55 220–225.Google Scholar
  8. Bélanger, D., & Tétreau, B. L'influence d'une motivation inappropriée sur le comportement du rat et sa fréquence cardiaque. (Influence of irrelevant motivation on the behavior and heart rate of the rat.)Canadian Journal of Psychology 1961,15 6–14.Google Scholar
  9. Berlyne, D. E. A theory of human curiosity.British Journal of Psychology 1954,45 180–191. (a)Google Scholar
  10. Berlyne, D. E. An experimental study of human curiosity.British Journal of Psychology 1954,45 256–265. (b)Google Scholar
  11. Berlyne, D. E. The arousal and satiation of perceptual curiosity in the rat.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1955,48 238–246.Google Scholar
  12. Berlyne, D. E. Conflict and information-theory variables as determinants of human perceptual curiosity.Journal of Experimental Psychology 1957,53 399–404.Google Scholar
  13. Berlyne, D. E. The influence of albedo and complexity of visual stimuli on visual fixation in the human infant.British Journal of Psychology 1958,49 315–318. (a)Google Scholar
  14. Berlyne, D. E. The influence of complexity and novelty in visual figures on orienting responses.Journal of Experimental Psychology 1958,55 289–296. (b)Google Scholar
  15. Berlyne, D. E.Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1960.Google Scholar
  16. Berlyne, D. E. Conflict and the orientation reaction.Journal of Experimental Psychology 1961,62 476–483.Google Scholar
  17. Berlyne, D. E. Complexity and incongruity variables as determinants of exploratory choice and evaluative ratings.Canadian Journal of Psychology 1963,17 274–290. (a)Google Scholar
  18. Berlyne, D. E. Motivational problems raised by exploratory and epistemic behavior. In S. Koch (Ed.),Psychology—a study of a science (Vol. 5). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1963. Pp. 284–364. (b)Google Scholar
  19. Berlyne, D. E.Structure and direction in thinking. New York: Wiley, 1965.Google Scholar
  20. Berlyne, D. E. Curiosity and exploration.Science 1966,153 25–33.Google Scholar
  21. Berlyne, D. E. Behavior theory as personality theory. In E. F. Borgotta & W. W. Lambert (Eds.),Handbook of personality theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1968.Google Scholar
  22. Berlyne, D. E. The reward-value of different stimulation. In J. T. Trapp (Ed.),Reinforcement and behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1969.Google Scholar
  23. Berlyne, D. E.Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1971.Google Scholar
  24. Berlyne, D. E. Uniformity in variety: Extension to three-element visual patterns and to nonverbal measures.Canadian Journal of Psychology 1972,26 277–291. (a)Google Scholar
  25. Berlyne, D. E. Ends and means of experimental aesthetics.Canadian Journal of Psychology 1972,26 303–325. (b)Google Scholar
  26. Berlyne, D. E. Experimental aesthetics. In P. C. Dodwell (Ed.),New horizons in psychology (Vol. 2). Harmondsworth, London: Penguin, 1972. (c)Google Scholar
  27. Berlyne, D. E. Reinforcement values of visual patterns compared through concurrent performances.Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior 1972,18 281–285. (d)Google Scholar
  28. Berlyne, D. E. (Ed.).Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation. Washington, D.C.: Hemisphere, 1974. (a)Google Scholar
  29. Berlyne, D. E. Information and motivation. In A. Silverstein (Ed.),Human communication: Theoretical explorations. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1974. (b)Google Scholar
  30. Berlyne, D. E. Behaviorism? Cognitive theory? Humanistic psychology? To Hull with them all.Canadian Psychological Review 1975,16 69–80.Google Scholar
  31. Berlyne, D. E., & Borsa, D. M. Uncertainty and the orientation reaction.Perception and Psychophysics 1968,3 77–79.Google Scholar
  32. Berlyne, D. E., Craw, M. A., Salapatek, P. H., & Lewis, J. L. Novelty, complexity, incongruity, extrinsic motivation and the GSR.Journal of Experimental Psychology 1963,66 560–567.Google Scholar
  33. Berlyne, D. E., & Crosier, J. B. Effects of complexity and pre-choice stimulation on exploratory choice.Perception and Psychophysics 1971,10 242–246.Google Scholar
  34. Berlyne, D. E., & McDonnell, P. Effects of stimulus complexity and incongruity on duration of EEG desynchronization.Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1965,18 156–161.Google Scholar
  35. Berlyne, D. E., & Parham, L. C. C. Determinants of subjective novelty.Perception and Psychophysics 1968,3 415–423.Google Scholar
  36. Berlyne, D. E., Salapatek, P. H., Gelman, R. S., & Zener, S. L. Is light increment really rewarding to the rat?Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1964,58 148–151.Google Scholar
  37. Bexton, W. A., Heron, W., & Scott, T. H. Effects of decreased variation in the sensory environment.Canadian Journal of Psychology 1954,8 70–76.Google Scholar
  38. Blum, G. S., Geiwitz, P. J., & Stewart, C. G. Cognitive arousal: The evolution of a model.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1967,5 138–151.Google Scholar
  39. Bolles, R. C.Theory of motivation. New York: Harper & Row, 1967.Google Scholar
  40. Bonvallet, M., Dell, P., & Hiebel, G. Tonus sympathique et activité électrique corticale.E. E. G. and Clinical Neurophysiology 1954,6 103–118.Google Scholar
  41. Bradley, P. B. The central action of certain drugs in relation to the reticular formation of the brain. In H. H. Jasper et al. (Eds.),Reticular formation of the brain Boston: Little, Brown, 1958. Pp. 123–129.Google Scholar
  42. Bühler, C., Hetzer, H., & Mabel, F. Die Affektwirksamkeit von Fremdheitseindrücken im ersten Lebensjahr.Zeitschrift für Psychologie 1928 (Abt. 1),107 30–49.Google Scholar
  43. Burt, C.The factor analysis of the mind: An introduction to factor analysis in psychology. New York: Macmillan, 1941.Google Scholar
  44. Butler, R. A. Discrimination learning by rhesus monkeys to visual-exploration motivation.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1953,46 95–98.Google Scholar
  45. Butler, R. A. Incentive conditions which influence visual exploration.Journal of Experimental Psychology 1954,48 19–23. (a)Google Scholar
  46. Butler, R. A. Curiosity in monkeys.Scientific American 1954,190(2), 70–75. (b)Google Scholar
  47. Butler, R. A. The effect of deprivation of visual incentives on visual-exploration motivation in monkeys.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1957,50 177–179.Google Scholar
  48. Campbell, B. A., & Sheffield, F. D., Relation of random activity to food deprivation.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1953,46 320–322.Google Scholar
  49. Cannon, W. B.The wisdom of the body. New York: W. W. Norton, 1932.Google Scholar
  50. Chomsky, N.Language and mind. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1968.Google Scholar
  51. Crozier, J. B. Verbal and exploratory responses to sound sequences of varying complexity.Dissertation Abstracts International 1974 (July),35(I-B), 537.Google Scholar
  52. Darrow, C. W., Pathman, J., & Kronenberg, G. Level of autonomic activity and the electroencephalogram.Journal of Experimental Psychology 1946,36 355–365.Google Scholar
  53. Dashiell, J. F. A quantitative demonstration of animal drive.Journal of Comparative Psychology 1925,5 205–208.Google Scholar
  54. Day, H. I. Evaluations of subjective complexity, pleasingness and interestingness for a series of random polygons varying in complexity.Perception and Psychophysics 1967,2(7), 281–286.Google Scholar
  55. DeBold, R. G., Miller, N. E., & Jeensen, D. D. Effect of strength of drive determined by a new technique for classical appetitive conditioning of rats.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1965,59 102–108.Google Scholar
  56. Dell, P. C. Humoral effects on the brain stem reticular formations. In H. H. Jasper et al. (Eds.),Reticular formation of the brain. Boston: Little, Brown, 1958, Pp. 365–379.Google Scholar
  57. Dewes, P. B., & Morse, W. N. Behavioral pharmacology.Annual Review of Pharmacology 1961,1 145–174.Google Scholar
  58. Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E.Personality and psychotherapy: An analysis in terms of learning, thinking and culture. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950.Google Scholar
  59. Douglas, R. J. Cues for spontaneous alternation.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1966,62 171–183.Google Scholar
  60. Ducharme, R., & Bélanger, D. Influence d'une stimulation électrique sur le niveau d'activation et la performance. (Influence of electrical stimulation on general activation and performance.)Canadian Journal of Psychology 1961,15 61–68.Google Scholar
  61. Duffy, E. Emotion: An example of the need for reorientation in psychology.Psychological Review 1934,41 184–198.Google Scholar
  62. Duffy, E. The conceptual categories of psychology: A suggestion for revision.Psychological Review 1941,48 177–203. (a)Google Scholar
  63. Duffy, E. An explanation of “emotional” phenomena without the use of the concept “emotion.”Journal of General Psychology 1941,25 283–293. (b)Google Scholar
  64. Duffy, E. The psychological significance of the concept of “arousal” or “activation.”Psychological Review 1957,64 265–275.Google Scholar
  65. Duffy, E.Activation and behavior. New York: Wiley, 1962.Google Scholar
  66. Elliott, R. Physiological activity and performance: A comparison of kindergarten children with young adults.Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 1964,78 (10, Whole No. 587).Google Scholar
  67. Estes, W. K. Stimulus-response theory of drive. In M. R. Jones (Ed.),Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1958. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1958, Pp. 35–69.Google Scholar
  68. Eysenck, H. J.The structure of human personality. New York: Wiley, 1953.Google Scholar
  69. Eysenck, H. J.The biological basis of personality. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C Thomas, 1967.Google Scholar
  70. Fantz, R. L. Pattern vision in young infants.Psychological Record 1958,8 43–47.Google Scholar
  71. Fechner, G. T. Zur Experimentellen Aesthetik.Abhandlungen der Königliche Sächsische Gesellschaft der Wissenchaften. Mathematisch-physische Klasse 1871,9 555–635.Google Scholar
  72. Fechner, G. T.Vorschule der Aesthetik. Leipzig: Breithopf & Härtel, 1876.Google Scholar
  73. Feldman, S. M., & Waller, H. J. Dissociation of electrocortical activation and behavioral arousal.Nature 1962,196 1320–1322.Google Scholar
  74. Fiske, D. W., & Maddi, S. R. (Eds.).Functions of varied experience. Homewood, Illinois: Dorsey, 1961.Google Scholar
  75. Fowler, H.Curiosity and exploratory behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1965.Google Scholar
  76. Fowler, H. Satiation and curiosity. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.),Psychology of learning and motivation. New York: Academic Press, 1967.Google Scholar
  77. Freeman, G. L.The energetics of human behavior. Ithaca: Cornell University Press, 1948.Google Scholar
  78. Freud, S.Die Traumdeutung. Leipzig & Wien: Deuticke, 1900.Google Scholar
  79. Freud, S. Triebe und Triebschicksale.International Zeitschrift für Aerztliche Psychoanalysis 1915,3 84–100.Google Scholar
  80. Gale, A., Bramley, P., Lucas, B., & Christie, B. Differential effect of visual and auditory complexity on the E.E.G.: Negative hedonic value as a crucial variable?Psychonomic Science 1972,27(1), 21–24.Google Scholar
  81. Godkewitsch, M.Verbal, exploratory and physiological responses to stimulus properties underlying humor. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Toronto, 1974.Google Scholar
  82. Graham, F. K., & Clifton, R. K. Heart rate change as a component of the orienting response.Psychological Bulletin 1966,65(5), 305–320.Google Scholar
  83. Grastyán, E., Lissak, K., & Kékesi, F. Facilitation and inhibition of conditioned alimentary and defensive reflexes by stimulation of the hypothalamus and reticular formation.Acta Physiologica Hungarian 1956,9 133–151.Google Scholar
  84. Grossman, S. P. The physiological basis of specific and nonspecific motivational processes. In W. J. Arnold (Ed.),Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1968. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1968. Pp. 1–46.Google Scholar
  85. Harlow, H. F. Learning and satiation of response in intrinsically motivated complex puzzle performance by monkeys.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1950,43 289–294.Google Scholar
  86. Harlow, H. F. Learning by Rhesus monkeys on the basis of manipulation-exploration motives.Science 1953,117 466–467.Google Scholar
  87. Harlow, H. F., Harlow, M. K., & Meyer, D. R. Learning motivated by a manipulative drive.Journal of Experimental Psychology 1950,40 228–234.Google Scholar
  88. Hebb, D. O.The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley, 1949.Google Scholar
  89. Hebb, D. O. Drives and the C.N.S. (Conceptual nervous system).Psychological Review 1955,62 243–254.Google Scholar
  90. Hugelin, A., & Bonvallet, M. Tonus cortical et contrôle de la facilitation motrice d'origine réticulaire.Journal de Physiologie Pathologique et Géneral 1957,49 1171–1200.Google Scholar
  91. Hull, C. L. Goal attraction and directing ideas conceived as habit phenomena.Psychological Review 1931,38 487–506.Google Scholar
  92. Hull, C. L. The rat's speed-of-locomotion gradient in the approach to food.Journal of Comparative Psychology 1934,7 393–422.Google Scholar
  93. Hull, C. L.Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1943.Google Scholar
  94. Jones, A. Information deprivation in humans. In B. A. Maher (Ed.),Progress in experimental personality research (Vol. 4). New York: Academic Press, 1966.Google Scholar
  95. Jones, A., Wilkinson, H. J., & Braden, I. Information deprivation as a motivational variable.Journal of Experimental Psychology 1961,62 126–137.Google Scholar
  96. Kahneman, D.Attention and effort. New York: Prentice-Hall, 1973.Google Scholar
  97. Karmel, B. Z., Hoffmann, R. F., & Fegy, M. J. Processing of contour information by human infants evidenced by pattern-dependent evoked potentials.Child Development 1974,45(1), 39–48.Google Scholar
  98. Karsten, A. Psychische Sättigung.Psychologische Forschung 1928,10 142–254.Google Scholar
  99. Kesner, R. P., & Vredenburg, E. W. Effects of mid-brain reticular lesions upon the orienting response in the rat.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1970,73 170–174.Google Scholar
  100. Lacey, J. I. Somatic response patterning and stress: Some revisions of activation theory. In M. H. Appley & R. Trumbull (Eds.),Psychological stress: Issues in research. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967.Google Scholar
  101. Lacey, J. I., & Lacey, B. C. Verification and extension of the principle of autonomic response stereotypy.American Journal of Psychology 1958,71 50–73.Google Scholar
  102. Laffal, J. Response faults in word association as a function of response entropy.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 1955,50 265–270.Google Scholar
  103. Lanier, L. H. An experimental study of “affective conflict.”Journal of Psychology 1941,11 199–217.Google Scholar
  104. Leckhart, B. T., Levine, J. R., Goscinski, C., & Brayman, W. Duration of attention: The perceptual deprivation effect.Perception and Psychophysics 1970,7 163–164.Google Scholar
  105. Lindsley, D. B. Emotion. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.),Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951.Google Scholar
  106. Lindsley, D. B. Psychophysiology and motivation. In M. R. Jones (Ed.),Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1957. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1957.Google Scholar
  107. Luria, A. B.The working brain. Harmondsworth, London: Penguin, 1973.Google Scholar
  108. Mackintosh, N. J.The psychology of animal learning. London: Academic Press, 1974.Google Scholar
  109. Malmo, R. B. Measurement of drive: An unsolved problem. In M. R. Jones (Ed.),Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1958. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1958. Pp. 229–265.Google Scholar
  110. Medlatousek, J., & Gerová, M. Autonomic components of the orienting reaction.Physiologica Bohemoslovenika 1960,9(4), 274–282.Google Scholar
  111. Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H.Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, 1960.Google Scholar
  112. Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J.Social learning and imitation. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1941.Google Scholar
  113. Montgomery, K. C. A test of two explanations of spontaneous alternation.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1952,45 287–293.Google Scholar
  114. Montgomery, K. C. Exploratory behavior as a function of “similarity” of stimulus situations.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1953,46 129–133. (a)Google Scholar
  115. Montgomery, K. C. The effect of the hunger and thirst drives upon exploratory behavior.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1953,46 315–319. (b)Google Scholar
  116. Montgomery, K. C. The effect of activity deprivation upon exploratory behavior.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1953,46 438–411. (c)Google Scholar
  117. Montgomery, K. C. The role of the exploratory drive in learning.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1954,47 60–64.Google Scholar
  118. Montgomery, K. C. The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation and exploratory behavior.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1955,48 254–260.Google Scholar
  119. Moruzzi, G., & Magoun, H. W. Brain stem reticular formation and the activation of the E.E.G.E.E.G. and Clinical Neurophysiology 1949,1 455–473.Google Scholar
  120. Mowrer, O. H. Preparatory set (expectancy): A determinant in motivation and learning.Psychological Review 1938,45 62–91.Google Scholar
  121. Murray, H. A.Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford University Press, 1938.Google Scholar
  122. Myers, A. K., & Miller, N. E. Failure to find a learned drive based on hunger: Evidence for learning motivated by “exploration.”Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1954,47 428–436.Google Scholar
  123. Nicki, R. M. Arousal increment and degree of complexity as incentive.British Journal of Psychology 1972,63(2), 165–171.Google Scholar
  124. Nissen, H. W. A study of exploratory behavior in the white rat by means of the obstruction method.Journal of Genetic Psychology 1930,37 361–376.Google Scholar
  125. Nunnally, J. C., Knott, P. D., Duchnowski, A., & Parker, R. Pupillary dilation as a general measure of activation.Perception & Psychophysics 1967,2(4), 149–155.Google Scholar
  126. Osgood, C. E. The nature and measurement of meaning.Psychological Bulletin 1952,49 197–237.Google Scholar
  127. Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H.The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1957.Google Scholar
  128. Pavlov, I. P.Conditioned reflexes. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1927.Google Scholar
  129. Popov, N. A. Observations electroencéphalographique sur les réactions corticales chez l'homme.Année Psychologique 1953,53 415–429.Google Scholar
  130. Premack, D., Collier, G., & Roberts, C. L. Frequency of light-contingent bar pressing as a function of the amount of deprivation for light.American Psychologist 1957,12 411.Google Scholar
  131. Pribram, K. H. On the neurology of thinking.Behavioral Science 1959,4 265–287.Google Scholar
  132. Pribram, K. H. A review of theory in physiological psychology.Annual Review of Psychology 1960,11 1–40.Google Scholar
  133. Pribram, K. H., & McGuiness, D. Arousal, activation and effort in the control of attention.Psychological Review 1975,82 116–149.Google Scholar
  134. Razran, G. The dominance-contiguity theory of the acquisition of classical conditioning.Psychological Review 1957,54 1–46.Google Scholar
  135. Richter, C. P. A behavioristic study of the activity of the rat.Comparative Psychology Monographs 1922,1 1–55.Google Scholar
  136. Richter, C. P. Animal behavior and internal drives.Quarterly Review of Biology 1927,2 307–343.Google Scholar
  137. Richter, C. P. Total self-regulatory functions in animals and human beings.Harvey Lectures 1942–1943,38 63–103.Google Scholar
  138. Richter, C. P. Biology of drives.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1947,40 129–134.Google Scholar
  139. Roberts, C. L., Marx, M. H., & Collier, G. Light onset and light offset as reinforcers for the albino rat.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1958,51 575–579.Google Scholar
  140. Robinson, J., & Gantt, W. H. The orienting reflex (questioning reaction): Cardiac, respiratory, salivary and motor components.Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin 1947,80 321–353.Google Scholar
  141. Salapatek, P. Visual scanning of geometric figures by the human newborn.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968,66 247–258.Google Scholar
  142. Schlosberg, H. A. Three dimensions of emotion.Psychological Review 1954,61 81–88.Google Scholar
  143. Schönpflug, W. Gedächtnis und generalisierte psychophysiologische Aktivierung.Zeitschrift für erziehungswissenschaftliche Forschung, 1969.Google Scholar
  144. Simon, H. A. Motivational and emotional controls of congition.Psychological Review 1967,74(1), 29–39.Google Scholar
  145. Skinner, B. F.Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1953.Google Scholar
  146. Skinner, B. F.Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957.Google Scholar
  147. Small, W. S. Notes on the psychic development of the young white rat.American Journal of Psychology 1899,11 80–100.Google Scholar
  148. Sokolov, E. N.Vospriiate i uslovny refleks. Moscow: University of Moscow Press, 1958. (Perception and the Conditioned Reflex, New York: Macmillan, 1963.)Google Scholar
  149. Sokolov, E. N. [A probabilistic model of perception.]Voprosky Psikhologii, 1960. (Translation inSoviet Psychology and Psychiatry, 1963,1, 28–36.)Google Scholar
  150. Sokolov, E. N. [Mechanisms of memory.] Moscow: Moscow University Press, 1969.Google Scholar
  151. Spence, K. W.Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1956.Google Scholar
  152. Sperry, R. W. Physiological plasticity and brain circuit theory. In H. F. Harlow & C. N. Woolsey (Eds.),Biological and biochemical bases of behavior. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press, 1958. Pp. 401–424.Google Scholar
  153. Spranger, E.Lebensformen: Geisteswissenschaftliche Psychologie und Ethik der Persönlichkeit (3. Aufl.) Halle a.s.: M. Niemeyer, 1922.Google Scholar
  154. Sutherland, J. W. System theoretic limits on the cybernetic paradigm.Behavioral Science 1975,20 191–200.Google Scholar
  155. Thayer, R. E. Measurement of activation through self-report.Psychological Reports 1967,20 663–678.Google Scholar
  156. Thorndike, E. L. Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals.Psychological Review, Monograph Supplement, 1898,2(8).Google Scholar
  157. Tolman, E. C.Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Appleton, 1932.Google Scholar
  158. Troland, L. T.The fundamentals of human motivation. Princeton: Van Nostrand, 1928.Google Scholar
  159. Unger, S. M. Habituation of the vaso-constrictive orienting reaction.Journal of Experimental Psychology 1964,67 11–18.Google Scholar
  160. Valentine, C. W.The experimental psychology of beauty. London: Methuen, 1962.Google Scholar
  161. Velden, M. An empirical test of Sokolov's entropy model of the orienting response.Psychophysiology 1974,11(6), 682–691.Google Scholar
  162. Vinogradova, O. S. [The role of the orientation reflex in the process of establishing conditioned reflex connections in man.] In E. K. Solokov (Ed.),Orientirovoshnyrefleks i voprosy vysshei nervnoi deiatel'nosti v norme i patologii. Moscow: Academy of Pedagogical Sciences, 1959.Google Scholar
  163. Vinogradova, O. S. [Dynamic classification of responses to sensory stimuli of hippocampal neurons.]Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deiatel'nosti 1965,15(3), 500–512.Google Scholar
  164. Vinogradova, O. S. [Study of habituation in the neurons of the rabbit caudate nucleus.]Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deiatel'nosti 1968,18(4), 671–680.Google Scholar
  165. Vinogradova, O. S., Konovalov, V. F., & Semenova, T. P. [Neural correlates of the extinction of the orienting reflex in various brain structures.]Voprosy Psikhologii 1969,15(1), 46–59.Google Scholar
  166. Voronin, L. G. et al. (Eds.). [The orienting reflex and orienting investigatory activity.] Moscow: RSFSR Academy of Pedagogical Sciences, 1958.Google Scholar
  167. Voronin, L. G., & Sokolov, E. N. [Skin galvanic reactions with pairing of two auditory stimuli.]Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deiatel'nosti 1960,10 3–9.Google Scholar
  168. Watson, J. B.Behaviorism. New York: W. W. Norton, 1924.Google Scholar
  169. Webb, W. B. The motivational aspect of an irrelevant drive in the behavior of the white rat.Journal of Experimental Psychology 1949,39 1–14.Google Scholar
  170. White, R. W. Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence.Psychological Review 1959,66 297–333.Google Scholar
  171. Wikler, A. Mechanisms of action of drugs that modify personality function.American Journal of Psychiatry 1952,108 590–599.Google Scholar
  172. Wilson, N. J., & Wilson, W. P. The duration of human electroencephalographic arousal responses elicited by photic stimulation.E.E.G. and Clinical Neurophysiology 1959,11 85–91.Google Scholar
  173. Woodworth, R. S.Dynamic psychology. New York: Columbia University Press, 1918.Google Scholar
  174. Wundt, W. [Lectures on human and animal psychology (2nd ed.)] (E. B. Titchener, Trans.). London: Allen, 1896.Google Scholar
  175. Yaremko, R. M., & Keleman, K. The orienting reflex and amount and direction of conceptual novelty.Psychonomic Science 1972,27(4), 195–196.Google Scholar
  176. Young, P. T.Motivation and emotion: A survey of the determinants of human and animal activity. New York: Wiley, 1961.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1978

Authors and Affiliations

  • Daniel E. Berlyne

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations