Advertisement

Journal of Chemical Ecology

, Volume 9, Issue 11, pp 1473–1481 | Cite as

Mushroom chemical defense: Food aversion learning induced by hallucinogenic toxin, muscimol

  • Scott Camazine
Article

Abstract

Wild animals eat fungi, yet mushroom poisonings in nature are unknown. The opossumDidelphis virginiana readily consumed the toxic mushroomAmanita muscaria, became ill, and then developed an aversion to the fungus. Both the illness and the aversion were due, in part at least, to the toxin muscimol. This appears to be the first demonstration of a mushroom chemical defense against fungivores and the first reported role in nature for an hallucinogen.

Key words

Mushroom antifeedant food aversion muscimol hallucinogen opossum Didelphis virginiana Amanita muscaria plant-herbivore coevolution 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Camazine, S.M., Resch, J.F., Eisner, T., andMeinwald, J. 1983. Mushroom chemical defense: Pungent sesquiterpenoid dialdehyde antifeedant to opossum.J. Chem. Ecol. 9:1439–1447.Google Scholar
  2. Chilton, W.S. 1978. Chemistry and mode of action of mushroom toxins, pp. 110–115,in B.H. Rumack and E. Salzman (eds.). Mushroom Poisoning: Diagnosis and Treatment. CRC Press, West Palm Beach, Florida.Google Scholar
  3. Domjan, M. 1977. Attenuation and enhancement of neophobia for edible substances, pp. 151–179,in L.M. Barker, M.R. Best, and M. Domjan, (eds). Learning Mechanisms in Food Selection. Baylor University Press, Texas.Google Scholar
  4. Edmunds, M. 1974. Defence in Animals. Longman, Essex.Google Scholar
  5. Eisner, T., andGrant, R.P. 1981. Toxicity, odor aversion, and “olfactory aposematism.”Science 213:476.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Fogel, R., andTrappe, J.M. 1978. Fungus consumption (mycophagy) by small animals.Northwest Sci. 52:1–31.Google Scholar
  7. Garb, J.L., andStunkard, A.J. 1974. Taste aversion in man.Am. J. Psychiatry 131:1204–1207.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Garcia, J., andHankins, W.G. 1977. On the origin of food aversion paradigms, pp. 3–19,in L.M. Barker, M.R. Best, and M. Domjan, (eds.). Learning Mechanisms in Food Selection. Baylor University Press, Texas.Google Scholar
  9. Garcia, J., andRusiniak, K.W. 1980. What the nose learns from the mouth, pp. 141–156, in D. Muller-Schwarze and R.M. Silverstein, (eds.). Chemical Signals. Plenum Press, New York.Google Scholar
  10. Gelperin, A. 1975. Rapid food-aversion learning by a terrestrial mollusk.Science 189:567–570.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Hatt, R.T. 1929. The red squirrel: its life history and habits, with special reference to the Adirondacks of New York and the Harvard Forest.Roosevelt Wild Life Ann. 2:11–146.Google Scholar
  12. Klugh, A.B. 1927. Ecology of the red squirrel.J. Mammal. 8:1–32.Google Scholar
  13. Lincoff, G., andMitchel, D.H. 1977. Toxic and Hallucinogenic Mushroom Poisoning. Van Nostrand Rheinhold Co., New York.Google Scholar
  14. Metcalf, M.M. 1925.Amanita muscaria in Maine.Science 61:567.Google Scholar
  15. Moulton, D.G. 1973. The use of animals in olfactory research, pp. 143–223,in W.I. Gay (ed.). Methods of Animal Experimentation, IV. Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  16. Nichol, A.A. 1938. Experimental feeding of deer.Univ. Ariz. Agric. Exp. Sta., Techn. Bull. 75:1–39.Google Scholar
  17. Wallace, T. 1976. Animal Behavior: the puzzle of flavor aversion.Science 193:989–991.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1983

Authors and Affiliations

  • Scott Camazine
    • 1
  1. 1.Section of Neurobiology and Behavior Division of Biological SciencesCornell UniversityIthaca

Personalised recommendations