Journal of Neurology

, Volume 241, Issue 7, pp 439–447 | Cite as

Could bradykinesia in Parkinson's disease simply be compensation?

  • J. G. Phillips
  • K. E. Martin
  • J. L. Bradshaw
  • R. Iansek
Original Communication

Abstract

Even normal movements can be slow and hesitant. To distinguish between bradykinesia and the simple slow inefficiency sometimes seen in normal movement, we matched the movement durations of 12 patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) and 12 age-matched controls and examined end-point accuracy, number of submovements, force inefficiency, and relative duration of acceleration and deceleration phases of movement. Subjects used an electronic pen which sampled pen-tip position at 200 Hz, and performed a sequence of drawing movements to nine targets (0.5, 1, or 2 cm diameter) upon a WACOM SD420 graphics tablet. Patients could be trained to move at the preferred speed of controls (and vice versa). When moving at the same fast speed as controls, patient's movements were less accurate (increased end-point spread). Even when moving at their own preferred speed, patients' movements were less efficient (more submovements, more zero crossings in acceleration function) than controls moving at the same speed. If bradykinesia simply reflected increased caution and visual guidance, we would expect patients to exhibit prolongeddecelerative phases of movement associated with terminal guidance. However, patients consistently required prolongedaccelerative phases of movement, suggesting that there was a problem in generating appropriate movement forces to produce the required end-point accuracy. It is hypothesised that bradykinesia is not simply a compensation for defective preparatory processes, but may reflect a defective internal cue in PD which disrupts and impairs the outflow of motor responses.

Key words

Bradykinesia Parkinson's disease Basal ganglia 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Beggs WDA, Howarth CI (1972) The accuracy of aiming at a target. Some further evidence for a theory of intermittent control. Acta Psychol (Amst) 36:171–177Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Berardelli A, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Marsden CD (1984) Movements not involved in posture are abnormal in Parkinson's disease. Neurosci Lett 47:47–50Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Berardelli A, Dick JPR, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Marsden CD (1986) Scaling of the size of the first agonist EMG burst during rapid wrist movements in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 49:1273–1279Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Bock O, Arnold K (1992) Motor control prior to movement onset: preparatory mechanisms for pointing at visual targets. Exp Brain Res 90:209–216Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Brotchie P, Iansek R, Horne MK (1991) The motor function of the monkey globus pallidus: cognitive aspects of movement and phasic neuronal activity. Brain 114:1685–1702Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Delwaide PJ, Gonce M (1988) Pathophysiology of Parkinson's signs. In: Jankovic J, Tolosa E (eds) Parkinson's disease and movement disorders. Urban & Schwarzenberg, Baltimore, pp 59–73Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Draper IT, Johns RS (1964) The disorders of movement in Parkinsonism and the effect of drug treatment. Bull Johns Hopkins Hosp 115:465–480Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Evarts EV, Teräväinen H, Calne DB (1981) Reaction time in Parkinson's disease. Brain 104:167–186Google Scholar
  9. 9.
    Flowers KA (1976) Visual ‘closedloop’ and ‘open-loop’ characteristics of voluntary movement in patients with Parkinsonism and intention tremor. Brain 99:269–310Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Glickstein M, Stein J (1991) Paradoxical movement in Parkinson's disease. Trends Neurosci 14:480Google Scholar
  11. 11.
    Hallett M (1990) Clinical neurophysiology of akinesia. Rev Neurol (Paris) 146:585–590Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Hallett M, Khoshbin S (1980) A physiological mechanism of bradykinesia. Brain 103:301–314Google Scholar
  13. 13.
    Hogan N, Flash T (1987) Moving gracefully: quantitative theories of motor coordination. Trends Neurosci 10:170–174Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Jankovic J (1987) Pathophysiology and clinical assessment of motor symptoms in Parkinson's disease. In: Koller WC (ed) Handbook of Parkinson's disease. Dekker, New York, pp 99–126Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Kokmen E, Smith GE, Peterson RC, Tangalos E, Ivnick RC (1991) The short test of mental status: correlations with standardized psychometric testing. Arch Neurol 48:725–728Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Marsden CD (1982) The mysterious motor function of the basal ganglia: the Robert Wartenberg Lecture. Neurology 32:514–539Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Meyer DE, Smith JEK, Kornblum S, Abrams RA, Wright CE (1991) Speed-accuracy tradeoffs in aimed movements: toward a theory of rapid voluntary action. In: Jeannerod M (ed) Attention and performance XIII. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N. J., pp 173–223Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Müller F, Abbs JH (1990) Precision grip in Parkinsonian patients. Adv Neurol 53:191–195Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Nagasaki H (1989) Asymmetric velocity and acceleration profiles of human arm movements. Exp Brain Res 74:319–326Google Scholar
  20. 20.
    Phillips JG, Stelmach GE, Teasdale N (1991) What can indices of handwriting quality tell us about Parkinsonian handwriting? Hum Mov Sci 10:301–314Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    Phillips JG, Bradshaw JL, Iansek R, Chin E (1993) Motor functions of the basal ganglia. Psychol Res 55:175–181Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Sheridan MR, Flowers KA, Hurrell J (1987) Programming and execution of movements in Parkinson's disease. Brain 110:1247–1271Google Scholar
  23. 23.
    Stelmach GE, Phillips JG (1992) Motor control in Parkinson's disease. In: Turnbull GI (ed) Physical therapy management of Parkinson's disease. Churchill Livingstone, New York, pp 37–48Google Scholar
  24. 24.
    Stelmach GE, Worringham CJ, Strand EA (1986) Movement preparation in Parkinson's disease: the use of advanced information. Brain 109:1179–1194Google Scholar
  25. 25.
    Stelmach GE, Teasdale N, Phillips J, Worringham CJ (1989) Force production characteristics in Parkinson's disease. Exp Brain Res 76:165–172Google Scholar
  26. 26.
    Teasdale N, Phillips J, Stelmach GE (1990) Temporal movement control in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 53:862–868Google Scholar
  27. 27.
    Webster DD (1968) Critical analysis of the disability in Parkinson's disease. Mod Treat 5:257–282Google Scholar
  28. 28.
    Yanagawa S, Shindo M, Yanagisawa N (1990) Muscular weakness in Parkinson's disease. Adv Neurol 53:259–269Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1994

Authors and Affiliations

  • J. G. Phillips
    • 1
  • K. E. Martin
    • 1
  • J. L. Bradshaw
    • 1
  • R. Iansek
    • 2
  1. 1.Psychology DepartmentMonash UniversityClaytonAustralia
  2. 2.Neurosciences DepartmentMonash Medical CentreClaytonAustralia

Personalised recommendations