Advertisement

Journal of Behavioral Medicine

, Volume 16, Issue 6, pp 589–610 | Cite as

Parent-infant cosleeping: The appropriate context for the study of infant sleep and implications for sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) research

  • Sarah Mosko
  • James McKenna
  • Michael Dickel
  • Lynn Hunt
Article

Abstract

Nearly all laboratory research on human infant sleep assumes that solitary sleeping is the normal and desirable environment. Yet solitary sleeping in infancy is a very recent custom limited to Western industrialized societies, and most of the world's peoples still practice parent-infant cosleeping. A hypothesis is presented that cosleeping provides a sensory-rich environment which is the more appropriate environment in which to study normal infant sleep. In addition, two preliminary, in-laboratory, polygraphic investigations of mother-infant cosleeping are reported in normal infants within the peak age range for sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS). Five mother-infant pairs coslept for 1 night in the first study; in the second, three additional pairs slept separately for 2 nights and coslept the third consecutive night. The results suggest that cosleeping is associated with enhanced infant arousals and striking temporal overlap in infant and maternal arousals. Infant sleep also showed subtle alterations with cosleeping, as manifested in increased overlap with corresponding maternal sleep stages and decreased amount of Stage 3–4. These are the first in-laboratory investigations of parent-infant cosleeping. The implications of the hypothesis and preliminary results for research on the normal development of infant sleep and on SIDS are discussed.

Key words

infant parent-infant cosleeping sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Agnew, H. W., Jr., Webb, V. B., and Williams, R. L. (1966). The first night effect: An EEG study of sleep.Psychophysiology, 2: 263–266.Google Scholar
  2. Anders, T. F., and Keener, M. (1985). Developmental course of nighttime sleep-wake patterns in full-term and premature infants during the first year of life. I.Sleep 8: 173–192.Google Scholar
  3. Ariagno, R. L., and Glotzbach, S. F. (1991). Sudden infant death syndrome. In Rudolph, A. M. (ed.),Pediatrics, 19th ed., Appleton-Lange, Norwalk, pp. 850–858.Google Scholar
  4. Ashton, R. (1973). The influence of state and prandial condition upon the reactivity of the newborn to auditory stimuli.J. Exp. Child Psychol. 15: 315–327.Google Scholar
  5. Barry, H., III, and Paxson, L. M. (1971). Infancy and early childhood: cross-cultural codes. 2.Ethology 10: 466–508.Google Scholar
  6. Ben-Shaul, D. M. (1962). The composition of the milk of wild animals.Int. Zool. Year Book 4: 333–342.Google Scholar
  7. Bernstein, P., Emde, R., and Campos, J. (1973). REM sleep in four-month infants under home and laboratory conditions.Psychosomat. Med. 35: 322–329.Google Scholar
  8. Bowes, G., Woolf, G. M., Sullivan, C. E., and Phillipson E. A. (1980). Effect of sleep fragmentation on ventilatory and arousal responses of sleeping dogs to respiratory stimuli.Am. Rev. Resp. Dis. 122: 899–908.Google Scholar
  9. Bowlby, J. (1969).Attachment and Loss, Vol. 1. Attachment, Hogarth Press, London.Google Scholar
  10. Brady, J. P., Chir, B., and McCann, E. M. (1985). Control of ventilation in subsequent siblings of victims of sudden infant death syndrome.J. Pediat. 106: 212–217.Google Scholar
  11. Burton, R. V., and Whiting, J. W. M. (1961). The absent father and cross-sex identity.Merrill-Palmer Q. 7: 85–95.Google Scholar
  12. Busby, K., and Pivik, R. T. (1983). Failure of high intensity auditory stimuli to affect behavioral arousal in children during the first sleep cycle.Pediat. Res. 17: 802–805.Google Scholar
  13. Carskadon, M. A., Brown, E. D., and Dement, W. C. (1982). Sleep fragmentation in the elderly: Relationship to daytime sleep tendency.Neurobiol. Aging 3: 321–327.Google Scholar
  14. Caudill, W., and Plath, D. (1966). Who sleeps by whom? Parent-child involvement in urban Japanese families. In Levine, R. (ed.),Culture and Personality, Aldine Press, Chicago, pp. 25–154.Google Scholar
  15. Cernoch, J. M., and Porter, R. H. (1985). Recognition of maternal axillary odors by infants.Child Dev. 56: 1593–1598.Google Scholar
  16. Challamel, M.-J., Revol, M., Leszczynski, M.-C., and Debilly, G. (1981). Organisation nycthemerale de etats de vigilance chez le nourrisson normal et le nourrisson dit “rechappe d'un syndrome de mort subite”.Rev. EEG Neurophysiol. 11: 28–36.Google Scholar
  17. Coons, S., and Guilleminault, C. (1985). Motility and arousal in near miss sudden infant death syndrome.J. Pediatr. 107: 728–732.Google Scholar
  18. Davis, D. P. (1985). Cot death in Hong Kong: A rare problem?Lancet 2: 1346–1348.Google Scholar
  19. DeVore, I., and Konner, M. J. (1974). Infancy in hunter-gatherer life: An ethological perspective. In White, N. (ed.),Ethology and Psychiatry, University of Toronto Press, Toronto, pp. 255–269.Google Scholar
  20. Elias, M. F., Nicholson, N. A., Bora, C., and Johnston, J. (1986). Sleep/wake patterns of breast-fed infants in the first 2 years of life.Pediatrics 3: 322–329.Google Scholar
  21. Fleming, P. J., Gilbert, R., Azaz, Y., Berry, P. J., Rudd, P. T., Stewart, A., and Hall, E. (1990). Interaction between bedding and sleeping position in the sudden infant death syndrome: A population based case-control study.Br. Med. J. 301: 85–89.Google Scholar
  22. Glotzbach, S. G., and Heller, H. S. (1989). Thermoregulation. In Kryger, M. H., Roth, T., and Dement, W. C. (eds.),Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, W. B. Saunders, Philadelphia, pp. 300–309.Google Scholar
  23. Goodall, J. (1986).The Chimpanzees of Gombe. Patterns of Behavior, Belknap Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  24. Guilleminault, C., and Souquet, M. (1979). Sleep states and related pathology. In Korobkin, R., and Guilleminault, C. (eds.),Adv. Perinatal Neural., Vol. 1, S. P. Medical and Scientific Books, New York. pp. 225–247.Google Scholar
  25. Guntheroth, W. G. (1989).Crib Death: The Sudden Infant Death Syndrome, Futura, Mount Kisco, NY.Google Scholar
  26. Guntheroth, W. G., and Spiers, P. S. (1992). Sleeping prone and the risk of sudden infant death syndrome.JAMA 267: 2359–2363.Google Scholar
  27. Haddad, G. G., Leistner, H. L., Epstein, R. A., Epstein, M. A. F., Grodin, W. K., and Mellins R. B. (1980). CO2-induced changes in ventilation and ventilatory pattern in normal sleeping infants.J. Appl. Physiol. 48: 684–688.Google Scholar
  28. Harper, R. M., Leake, B., Hoffman, H., Walter, D. O., Hoppenbrouwers, T., Hodgman, J., and Sterman, M. B. (1981a). Periodicity of sleep states is altered in infants at risk for the sudden infant death syndrome.Science 213: 1030–1032.Google Scholar
  29. Harper, R. M., Leake, B., Miyahara, L., Mason, J., Hoppenbrouwers, T., Sterman, M. B., and Hodgman, J. (1981b). Temporal sequencing in sleep and waking states during the first 6 months of life.Exp. Neural. 72: 294–307.Google Scholar
  30. Hoppenbrouwers, T., Hodgman, J. E., Arakawa, K., Geidel, S. A., and Sterman, M. B. (1988). Sleep and waking states in infancy: Normative studies.Sleep 11: 387–401.Google Scholar
  31. Hoppenbrouwers, T., Hodgman, J. E., Arakawa, K., and Sterman, M. B. (1989). Polysomnographic sleep and waking states are similar in subsequent siblings of SIDS and control infants during the first six months of life.Sleep 12: 265–276.Google Scholar
  32. Kahn, A., Picard, E., and Blum, D. (1986). Auditory arousal thresholds of normal and near-miss SIDS infants.Dev. Med. Child Neural. 28: 299–302.Google Scholar
  33. Konner, M. J. (1981). Evolution of human behavior development. In Monroe, R., and Whiting, B. (eds.),Handbook of Cross-Cultural Human Development, Garland Press, New York, pp. 3–51.Google Scholar
  34. Konner, M., and Worthman, C. (1980). Nursing frequency, gonadal function, and birth spacing among!Kung hunter-gathers.Science 207: 788–791.Google Scholar
  35. Lancaster, J. B., and Lancaster, C. S. (1983). Parental investment: The hominid adaptation. In Ortner, D. J. (ed.),How Humans Adapt: A Biocultural Odyssey, Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC, pp. 33–56.Google Scholar
  36. Lee, N. Y., Chan, Y. F., Davies, D. P., Lau, E., and Yip, D. C. P. (1989). Sudden infant death syndrome in Hong Kong: Confirmation of low incidence.Br. Med. J. 298: 721.Google Scholar
  37. Lozoff, B., and Brittenham, G. (1979). Infant care: Cache or carry.J. Pediat. 95: 478–483.Google Scholar
  38. Lozoff, B., Wolf, A. W., and Davis, N. S. (1984). Cosleeping in urban families with young children in the United States.Pediatrics 74: 171–182.Google Scholar
  39. MacFarlane, A. J. (1975). Olfaction in the development of social preferences in the human neonate.Parent-Infant Interaction, Ciba Found. Symp. 33: 103–117.Google Scholar
  40. McCulloch, K., Brouillette, R. T., Guzzetta, A. J., and Hunt, C. E. (1982). Arousal responses in near-miss sudden infant death syndrome and in normal infants. J. Pediat. 101: 911–917.Google Scholar
  41. McKenna, J. J. (1986). An anthropological perspective on sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS): The role of parental breathing cues and speech breathing adaptations.Med. Anthropol. 10: 9–53.Google Scholar
  42. McKenna, J. J., and Mosko, S. (1990). Evolution and the sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS). III. Infant arousal and parent-infant co-sleeping.Hum. Nature 1: 291–330.Google Scholar
  43. McKenna, J. J., Mosko, S., Dungy, C., McAninch, J. (1990). Sleep and arousal patterns of cosleeping human mother-infant pairs. A preliminary physiological study with implications for the study of sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS).Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 83: 331–347.Google Scholar
  44. Mitchell, E. A. (1993). Update of the New Zealand cot death study (in press).Google Scholar
  45. Parmelee, A. H., Wenner, W. H., and Schulz, H. R. (1984). Infant sleep patterns: From birth to 16 weeks of age.J. Pediat. 65: 576–582.Google Scholar
  46. Rechtschaffen, A., and Kales, A. (1968).A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques and Scoring Systems for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects, BIS/BRI, UCLA, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
  47. Russel, M. J. (1976). Human olfactory communication.Nature 260: 520–522.Google Scholar
  48. Schaal, B., Montagner, H., Hertling, E., Bolzoni, D., Moyse, A., and Quichon, R. (1980). Les stimulations olfactives dans les relations entre l'enfant et la mere.Reprod. Nutr. Dev. 20: 843–858.Google Scholar
  49. Schwartz, P. J., and Sagantini, A. (1988). Cardiac innervation, neonatal electrocardiology, and SIDS. A key for a novel preventive strategy. InThe Sudden Infant Death Syndrome: Cardiac and Respiratory Mechanisms and Interventions, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., Vol. 533, pp. 210–220.Google Scholar
  50. Schwartz, P. J., Southall, D. P., and Valdes-Dapena, M. (1988).The Sudden Infant Death Syndrome. Cardiac and Respiratory Mechanisms and Interventions, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., Vol. 533.Google Scholar
  51. Sostek, A. M., and Anders, T. F. (1975). Effects of varying laboratory conditions on behavioral conditions or behavioral-state organization in two- and eight-week old infants.Child Dev. 46: 871–878.Google Scholar
  52. Stewart, M. W., and Stewart, L. A. (1991). Modification of sleep respiratory patterns by auditory stimulation: Indications of a technique for preventing sudden infant death syndrome?Sleep 14: 241–248.Google Scholar
  53. Sullivan, R. M., Taborsky-Barba, S., Mendoza, R., Itano, A., Leon, M., Cotman, C. W., Payne, T. F., and Lott, I. (1991). Olfactory classical conditioning in neonates.Pediatrics 87: 511–517.Google Scholar
  54. Super, C. M., and Harkness, S. (1982). The infant's niche in rural Kenya and metropolitan America. In Adler, L. L. (ed.),Cross-Cultural Research at Issue, Academic Press, New York, pp. 47–55.Google Scholar
  55. Takeda, K. (1987). A possible mechanism of sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS).J. Kyoto Pref. Univ. Med. 96: 965–968.Google Scholar
  56. Tasaki, H., Yamashita, M., and Miyazaki, S. (1988). The incidence of SID in Saga Prefecture (1981–1985).J. Pediat. Assoc. Jap. 92: 364–368.Google Scholar
  57. Thevenin, T. (1976).The Family Bed, Thevenin, Minneapolis.Google Scholar
  58. Trevathan, W. R. (1987).Human Birth: An Evolutionary Perspective, Aldine de Gruyter, New York.Google Scholar
  59. Valsiner, J. (1989).Human Development and Culture: The Social Nature of Personality and Its Study, Lexington Books, Lexington, MA.Google Scholar
  60. Whiting, B. B. (1963).Six Cultures: Studies of Child Rearing, John Wiley & Sons, New York.Google Scholar
  61. Williams, H. L., Hammack, J. T., Daly, R. L., Dement, W. C., and Lubin, A. (1964). Responses to auditory stimulation, sleep loss and the EEG stages of sleep.Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol. 16: 269–279.Google Scholar
  62. Willinger, M. (1989). SIDS—A challenge.J. NIH Res. 1: 73–80.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Sarah Mosko
    • 1
  • James McKenna
    • 1
  • Michael Dickel
    • 1
  • Lynn Hunt
    • 1
  1. 1.University of California Irvine Medical CenterOrange

Personalised recommendations