Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology

, Volume 26, Issue 6, pp 287–292 | Cite as

Distribution of hallucinations in the population

  • A. Y. Tien
Article

Summary

Hallucinations are often manifestations of severe psychiatric conditions seen clinically. However, unde is known about the distribution of incident hallucinations in the community, nor whether there has been a change over the past century. Data from the NIMH Epidemiologic Catchment Area Program is used here to provide descriptive information on the community distribution, and data from the Sidgewick study from a century earlier provides comparative information. In the ECA data, the incidence of visual hallucinations was slightly higher in males (about 20 per 1000 per year) than females (about 13 per 1000 per year) across the age span from 18 to 80 years old, with a subsequent increase in the rate for females (up to about 40 per 1000 per year) after age 80. For auditory hallucinations there was an age 25–30 peak in males with a trough for females, and a later age 40–50 peak for females. Overall, there were substantial gender differences, and the effect of aging to increase the incidence of hatlucinations was the most consistent and prominent. The Sidgewick study showed a much higher proportion of visual hallucinations than the ECA program. This might be due to factors affecting brain function as well as social and psychological changes over time, although methodological weaknesses in both studies might also be responable

References

  1. 1.
    Assad G, Shapiro B (1986) Hallucinations: theoretical and clinical overview. Am J Psychiatry 1088–1097Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Sidgewick H, Johnson A, Myers FWH et al. (1894) Report on the census of hallucinations. Proc Soc Psychic Res 34: 25–394Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    West DJ (1948) A mass-observation questionnaire on hallucinations. J Soc Psychic Res 34: 187–196Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Posey TB, Losch ME (1983) Auditory hallucinations of hearing voices in 375 normal subjects. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality 3: 99–113Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Eaton WW, Kessler LG (eds) (1985) Epidemiologic field methods in psychiatry: the NIMH Epidemiologic Catchment Area Program. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Robins LN, Helzer JE, Croughan J, Ratcliff KA (1981) National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule: its history, characteristics, and validity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 38: 381–389Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    American Psychiatric Association (1980) diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 3rd edn. American Psychiatric Association, WashingtonGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Munson ML, Orvaschel H, Skinner EA et al. (1985) Interviewers: characteristics, training, and field work. In: Eaton WW, Kessler LG (eds) Epidemiologic field methods in psychiatry: the NIMH Epidemiologic Catchment Area Program. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Eaton WW, Kramer M, Anthony JC et al (1989) The incidence of specific DIS/DSM-II mental disorders: data from the NIMH Epidemiologic Catchment Area Program Acta Psychiatr Scand 79: 163–178Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Hastie T, Tibsharani R (1990) Generalized additive models. Chapman & Hall, LondobGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Berrios GE, Brook P (1985) Delusions and the psychopathology of the elderly with dementia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 72: 296–301Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Cummings JL, Miller B, Hill MA et al (1987) Neuropsychiatric aspects of multi-infarct dementia and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol 44: 389–393Google Scholar
  13. 13.
    Link BG, Dohrenwend BP, Skodol AE (1986) Socio-economic status amd schizophrenia: noisome occupational characteristics as a risk factor. Am J Sociology 51: 242–258Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Berkson J (1946) Limitations of the application of fourfold table analysis to hospital data. Biometrics Bull 2: 47–53Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Muntaner C, Tien AY, Eaton WW, Garrison R. Occupational characteristics and the occurrence of psychotic disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 26: 273–280Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Jaynes J (1976) The origin of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind. Houghton Mifflin, BostonGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Burns A, Jacoby R, Levy R (1990) Psychiatric phenomena in Alzheimer's disease. II. Disorders of perception. Brit J Psych 157: 76–81Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Goldstein MJ (1987) Psychosocial issues. Schizophr Bull 13: 157–172Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Eaton WW, Day R, Kramer M (1988) In: Tsuang M, Simpson J (eds) Handbook of schizophrenia, vol. 3: Nosology, epidemiology and genetics. Elsevier, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Meehl PE (1989) Schizotaxia revisited. Arch Gen Psychiatry 46 935–944Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    Loranger AW (1984) Sex differences in age at onset of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psych 41: 157–161Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Lewine RRJ, Strauss JS, Gift TE (1981) Sex differences in age at first hospital admission for schizophrenia: fact or artifact? Am J Psych 138: 440–444Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • A. Y. Tien
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Mental HygieneThe Johns Hopkins University School of Hygiene and Public HealthBaltimoreUSA

Personalised recommendations